Introduction
In daily life, we often predict the consequences of our actions, which is best visible in situations where the expected consequence does not match the actual outcome. For example, when stairs are not as high as we predict, we tumble because the foot was not lifted high enough (Marigold et al.,
2007). With greater task experience, predictions become more accurate (Vater et al.,
2022). In sports, elite athletes can predict the actions of an opposing player very reliably (Loffing & Cañal-Bruland,
2017). In tennis, for example, prediction is particularly required under time pressure when both players are close to the net. In this situation, it would be difficult to reach the ball when reacting after the opponent hits the ball (Triolet et al.,
2013). Instead, players predict the likely shot direction based on visual cues about the posture of the opponent or knowledge about the player’s preferred shot direction (Abernethy et al.,
2001; Triolet et al.,
2013). Interestingly, these predictions are also reflected by the athlete’s gaze behavior. For example, in baseball, cricket, table tennis, and squash, predictive eye-movements are made to future location(s) along the ball’s trajectory (Bahill & LaRitz,
1984; Hayhoe et al.,
2012; Higuchi et al.,
2018; Land & Furneaux,
1997; Land & McLeod,
2000; Ripoll,
1989; Rodrigues et al.,
2002). If the ball bounces, these predictive saccades are generally initiated to the future bounce point. Yet, the functionality of these predictive saccades is still unclear (Diaz et al.,
2013; Mann et al.,
2013). Clearly, immediately after every predictive saccade, the relevant target object, in these cases the ball, is positioned in the peripheral visual field of the athlete. The goal of the present series of studies is to determine how functional predictive saccades are for processing peripheral information. Since gaze behavior in these situations consists largely of a pursuit eye-movement, followed by a (predictive) saccade, and a fixation after this saccade, we will compare motion perception during pursuit, fixation, and finally, in two ‘eye-cricket’ experiments, simulate the predictive saccades, with a combination of pursuit, saccade and fixation.
Cricket provides an ideal environment in which to understand predictive eye-movements. In cricket, a bowler bowls the ball in the direction of a batter. After the ball leaves the hand of the bowler, it bounces 2–10 m from the batter. The goal of a batter is then to hit the ball with their bat. The ball is released by a bowler with speeds up to 160 km/h (Croft et al.,
2008; Müller et al.,
2009), and the batter can have less than 600 ms from ball release to arrival (Sarpeshkar & Mann,
2011). Results by Renshaw and Fairweather (
2000) indicate that expert cricket players can use information from the postural cues of the bowler to discriminate different ball types, even before the ball is released. Müller et al. (
2009) showed that the ball-flight information between ball release and bounce is particularly important for batters to successfully intercept the ball after the bounce. Since expert cricket batsman only follow the ball for the first 100–150 ms of ball flight (Land & McLeod,
2000), they seem to pick up the relevant characteristics of the ball flight (i.e., speed and trajectory) very quickly.
During the ball flight when batting, players often initiate two predictive saccades: a first to the predicted point of ball-bounce, and a second to the predicted bat-ball contact point. The first saccade occurs after tracking the ball for the first portion of ball flight, around 100–200 ms ahead of the actual bounce depending on the ball speed (Land & McLeod,
2000). Interestingly, Land and McLeod (
2000), who investigated three players with different cricket expertise, found that a highly skilled player initiated their saccade earlier than the batters with lower skill level. This result, however, could not be replicated in a study with a higher number of high and low skilled players (Sarpeshkar et al.,
2017). The second predictive saccade is sometimes initiated to the place where the bat will make contact with the ball. This predictive saccade might be used because, as the ball is closer to the batsman, the large changes in viewing angles might make it difficult to use pursuit eye movements (Mann et al.,
2019). In one study, it has been reported that batters do not or cannot watch the ball when it is hit (Land & McLeod,
2000) yet a subsequent study has shown that they can by using the second predictive saccade (Mann et al.,
2013).
Mann et al. (
2013) proposed three potential functionalities for predictive saccades. First, predictive saccades may facilitate ball tracking after the ball bounce to a degree that is better than what would be possible when tracking the ball. The ball at bounce undergoes a considerable change in the angular velocity apparent at the eye, and it may be difficult for observers to pursue smoothly through this discontinuity in the motion path. Batsmen could anchor their gaze near the bounce location to avoid this discontinuity and facilitate tracking after the bounce (Mann et al.,
2013, p. 10, see also Brenner & Smeets,
2011; Diaz et al.,
2013; Hayhoe et al.,
2005; Mann et al.,
2013). Second, batters make predictions about where the ball will bounce and arrive, and predictive saccades to bounce could serve a purpose to check whether the predicted location of bounce aligns with the actual location of bounce in order to provide an accurate form of feedback for future predictions (Mann et al.,
2013, p. 10). Third, predictive saccades may aid “batters to better detect, and subsequently adapt to, unexpected changes in the flight-path of the ball after it bounces” for instance when bouncing off an irregular or rough surface (Mann et al.,
2013, p. 10).
It is possible that after predictive saccades, peripheral vision is used to make fast adjustments to the visually guided hitting action (Vater et al.,
2020b). For instance, despite not tracking the ball during the final phase of the ball flight after bounce, some cricket players have reported that they make late visually guided adjustments to their movements, for instance by adjusting their wrist orientation in the final phase hitting the ball (Mann et al.,
2013). If true, the batters must presumably be using their peripheral vision because they did not directly look at the ball. Similarly, in table tennis, elite athletes “anchor” their gaze at the expected hitting location and seem to use peripheral information to adjust their bat swing (Bootsma & van Wieringen,
1990). This finding supports the assumption that athletes process ball information after the predictive saccade, i.e., when the ball is in their peripheral vision. Similarly in a racquetball study, it was reported that predictive saccades toward bounce land slightly above (rather than on) the predicted ball-bounce location and are initiated 300–400 ms before the ball actually reaches the bounce (Diaz et al.,
2013). Since the updating of visual information takes only 80 ms (Diaz et al.,
2013), it is likely that information was updated using peripheral vision.
As could be seen so far, a typical gaze pattern in hitting sports consists of smooth pursuit eye movements (SPEMs), and saccades with subsequent fixations. When looking at fundamental research on SPEMs, humans are very accurate in following objects with their eyes and can adjust their eye-movement velocity to different speed perturbations (Gegenfurtner et al.,
2003). A similar accuracy is observed in psychometric performance (i.e., the ability to detect the perturbation, Braun et al.,
2010; Gegenfurtner et al.,
2003). In another study, it has been shown that short periods of pursuit tracking (between 100 and 300 ms) enable the prediction of whether an object will hit a target or not (Fooken et al.,
2016). In their study, participants (baseball players) had to track a virtual ball with their eyes and saw only the initial 100 ms, 200 ms, or 300 ms of its trajectory, after which participants were required to anticipate, using their index finger, when the ball would have hit a target zone. The results showed that more accurate SPEM tracking led to better predictions. That is, participants successfully used retinal information obtained during their SPEMs and presumably also extraretinal signals to predict the future location of the target (for a review see Fooken et al.,
2021). However, the duration of the initial target presentation period did not alter hitting accuracy. It could be that a combination of SPEMs and a saccade (as in sports) would be more beneficial than using pursuit alone when it comes to hitting accuracy.
After tracking the ball with the eyes, a batter’s eye-movements are characterized by a saccade ahead of a target. Basic research shows that saccades are pre-programmed and that visual attention is covertly shifted to the target of the saccade (for a review on eye-movements and selective attention see Souto & Kerzel,
2021). Saccades can also impair or even suppress information processing (for a review see Binda & Morrone,
2018). Observers are, for example, very poor in detecting image displacements during saccades (Bridgeman & Stark,
1991), or have distorted perception shortly before the onset of a saccade (Ross et al.,
1997). It is summarized that saccades (a) suppress visual sensitivity and dampen the sensation of motion, and (b) lead to a “gross perceptual distortion of visual space in anticipation of the repositioning of gaze” (Ross et al.,
2001, p. 113). If motion perception is impaired during saccades, it is unlikely that information processing occurs during the predictive saccade. Instead, batsman probably uses their peripheral vision to process visual information during the fixation that occurs after the predictive saccade.
This leads us to the third area of relevant basic research: the processing of motion in peripheral vision. When viewing motion changes with peripheral vision during fixation, motion changes can be detected across different viewing eccentricities and the time to detect these changes is independent of eccentricity—in contrast to SPEMs, where larger eccentricities lead to prolonged detection times (Vater et al.,
2020a,
2020b). Motion speed, however, is typically underestimated during fixation when using peripheral vision, and the magnitude of underestimation increases with eccentricity (Traschütz et al.,
2012). This result holds irrespective of the speed and contrast of stimuli (Hassan et al.,
2016). Thus, a moving ball viewed in peripheral vision should be perceived to be slower than it actually is. That means, when time-to-contact (TTC) is crucial, as it is the case when hitting a ball in cricket, TTC could be impaired because the ball is perceived slower than it actually is. Interestingly, research on the Aubert–Fleischl illusion (Aubert,
1886; Fleischl,
1882) found that an object appears to move slower when it is pursued than when it is viewed during fixation, leading to the opposite prediction (Fleischl,
1882).
When returning to the predicted functionalities of saccades by Mann et al. (
2013), and considering the reviewed literature, it may be that it is not the predictive saccade per se that facilitates or aids performance, but rather the fixation thereafter. Since the ball is viewed in peripheral vision during that fixation, peripheral monitoring could explain the use of predictive saccades. In a related study, Spering et al. (
2011) tested if the motion direction of a moving target is better predicted when using SPEM or fixation (using peripheral vision) and found better performance in the SPEM condition (for similar results see Bennett et al.,
2010; Brenner & Smeets,
2010; van Donkelaar & Lee,
1994). Our aim of the study is now to simulate predictive saccades, with a combination of pursuit, saccade and fixation. If saccades with subsequent fixations ahead of a target would indeed facilitate peripheral monitoring, better speed-change detection rates and TTC estimations should be found compared with SPEM alone conditions. Both of these hypotheses will be tested in this study. In the first two experiments, a SPEM of a moving target will be followed by a saccade and a subsequent fixation ahead of the target. To indicate a perceived speed change, participants were instructed to initiate a saccade to a fixation “+” and continue monitoring the target with peripheral vision and predict when the target would completely overlap with the “+” (TTC). In these two experiments, target occlusion times were systematically varied (Experiment 1: 1000 ms and 1500 ms; Experiment 2: 300 ms, 500 ms, 700 ms, 900 ms) to test whether longer peripheral monitoring times improve TTC performance. If peripheral vision is indeed used for target monitoring, better TTC estimations should be found when the target is occluded later (i.e., with longer monitoring time). In the third experiment (control experiment), participants will use SPEMs only and indicate the speed-change detection with a button press instead of a saccade. This allows us to directly compare the costs associated with predictive saccades in combination with peripheral monitoring. If saccades impair motion processing, better TTC estimations should be found in Experiment 3 compared to Experiment 2.