Skip to main content

Welkom bij THIM Hogeschool voor Fysiotherapie & Bohn Stafleu van Loghum

THIM Hogeschool voor Fysiotherapie heeft ervoor gezorgd dat je Mijn BSL eenvoudig en snel kunt raadplegen. Je kunt je links eenvoudig registreren. Met deze gegevens kun je thuis, of waar ook ter wereld toegang krijgen tot Mijn BSL. Heb je een vraag, neem dan contact op met helpdesk@thim.nl.

Registreer

Om ook buiten de locaties van THIM, thuis bijvoorbeeld, van Mijn BSL gebruik te kunnen maken, moet je jezelf eenmalig registreren. Dit kan alleen vanaf een computer op een van de locaties van THIM.

Eenmaal geregistreerd kun je thuis of waar ook ter wereld onbeperkt toegang krijgen tot Mijn BSL.

Login

Als u al geregistreerd bent, hoeft u alleen maar in te loggen om onbeperkt toegang te krijgen tot Mijn BSL.

Top
Gepubliceerd in:

02-06-2020 | Original Article

Generalization of novel sensorimotor associations among pianists and non-pianists: more evidence that musical training effects are constrained

Auteurs: Peter Q. Pfordresher, Chihiro Honda, Emma Greenspon, Karen Chow

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 5/2021

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

In the process of acquiring musical skills, such as playing the piano, we develop sensorimotor associations between motor movements and perception of pitch. Previous research suggests that these acquired associations are relatively inflexible and show limited generalizability to performance under novel conditions. The current study investigated whether piano training constrains the ability to generalize learning based on an unfamiliar (inverted) pitch mapping, using a transfer-of-training paradigm (Palmer and Meyer in Psychol Sci 11:63–68, 2000). Pianists and non-pianists learned a training melody by ear with normal (higher pitches to the right) or inverted (higher pitches to the left) pitch mapping. After training, participants completed a generalization test in which they listened to and then immediately reproduced four types of melodies that varied in their similarity to the melody used during training and were based on the same, a similar, an inverted, or a different pitch pattern. The feedback mapping during the generalization test matched training. Overall, pianists produced fewer errors and required fewer training trials than non-pianists. However, benefits of training were absent for pianists who trained with inverted feedback when they attempted to reproduce a melody with a different structure than the melody used for training. This suggests that piano experience may constrain one’s ability to generalize learning that is based on novel sensorimotor associations.
Voetnoten
1
It is worth noting that non-musicians in Western cultures typically report explicit knowledge about the mapping of pitch to piano keys. Our focus is not on this kind of explicit awareness, but instead on more implicit features of sensorimotor associations.
 
2
In Pfordresher & Chow (2019) only one successful trial was needed to progress. However, that study was limited by a high attrition rate (only 45% of recruited participants could complete the procedure). We reasoned that more repetitions of each progressive melody would facilitate memory consolidation. In support of this prediction, participant retention was much higher in this study (77%).
 
3
The pattern of results reported here remained the same when Log-transforming these data to better approximate a normal distribution.
 
4
The pattern of results reported here remained the same when analyzing the arc-sine square root transform of the data, yielding a closer approximation to the normal distribution.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Bangert, M., & Altenmüller, E. O. (2003). Mapping perception to action in piano practice: A longitudinal DC-EEG study. BMC Neuroscience, 4, 26.CrossRef Bangert, M., & Altenmüller, E. O. (2003). Mapping perception to action in piano practice: A longitudinal DC-EEG study. BMC Neuroscience, 4, 26.CrossRef
go back to reference Cohen, A., Ivry, R. B., & Keele, S. W. (1990). Attention and structure in sequence learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 17–30. Cohen, A., Ivry, R. B., & Keele, S. W. (1990). Attention and structure in sequence learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 17–30.
go back to reference Drost, U. C., Rieger, M., & Prinz, W. (2007). Instrument specificity in experienced musicians. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60, 527–533.CrossRef Drost, U. C., Rieger, M., & Prinz, W. (2007). Instrument specificity in experienced musicians. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60, 527–533.CrossRef
go back to reference Herholz, S. C., & Zatorre, R. J. (2012). Musical training as a framework for brain plasticity: Behavior, Function, and Structure. Neuron, 76, 486–502.CrossRef Herholz, S. C., & Zatorre, R. J. (2012). Musical training as a framework for brain plasticity: Behavior, Function, and Structure. Neuron, 76, 486–502.CrossRef
go back to reference Keller, P. E., & Koch, I. (2008). Action planning in sequential skills: Relations to music performance. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 275–291.CrossRef Keller, P. E., & Koch, I. (2008). Action planning in sequential skills: Relations to music performance. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 275–291.CrossRef
go back to reference Lahav, A., Saltzman, E., & Schlaug, G. (2007). Action representation of sound: Audiomotor recognition network while listening to newly acquired actions. The Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 308–314.CrossRef Lahav, A., Saltzman, E., & Schlaug, G. (2007). Action representation of sound: Audiomotor recognition network while listening to newly acquired actions. The Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 308–314.CrossRef
go back to reference Lidji, P., Kolinsky, R., Lochy, A., & Morais, J. (2007). Spatial associations for musical stimuli: A piano in the head? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33, 1189–1207.PubMed Lidji, P., Kolinsky, R., Lochy, A., & Morais, J. (2007). Spatial associations for musical stimuli: A piano in the head? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33, 1189–1207.PubMed
go back to reference MacKay, D. G., & Bowman, R. W. J. (1969). On producing the meaning in sentences. The American Journal of Psychology, 82, 23–39.CrossRef MacKay, D. G., & Bowman, R. W. J. (1969). On producing the meaning in sentences. The American Journal of Psychology, 82, 23–39.CrossRef
go back to reference Meyer, R. K., & Palmer, C. (2003). Temporal and motor transfer in music performance. Music Perception, 21, 81–104.CrossRef Meyer, R. K., & Palmer, C. (2003). Temporal and motor transfer in music performance. Music Perception, 21, 81–104.CrossRef
go back to reference Mishra, J. (2002). A qualitative analysis of strategies employed in efficient and inefficient memorization. Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music Education, 152, 74–86. Mishra, J. (2002). A qualitative analysis of strategies employed in efficient and inefficient memorization. Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music Education, 152, 74–86.
go back to reference Novembre, G., & Keller, P. E. (2014). A conceptual review on action-perception coupling in the musicians’ brain: what is it good for? Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 603.CrossRef Novembre, G., & Keller, P. E. (2014). A conceptual review on action-perception coupling in the musicians’ brain: what is it good for? Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 603.CrossRef
go back to reference Palmer, C., & Meyer, R. K. (2000). Conceptual and motor learning in music performance. Psychological Science, 11, 63–68.CrossRef Palmer, C., & Meyer, R. K. (2000). Conceptual and motor learning in music performance. Psychological Science, 11, 63–68.CrossRef
go back to reference Palmer, C., & Pfordresher, P. Q. (2003). Incremental planning in sequence production. Psychological Review, 110, 683–712.CrossRef Palmer, C., & Pfordresher, P. Q. (2003). Incremental planning in sequence production. Psychological Review, 110, 683–712.CrossRef
go back to reference Penhune, V. B. (2011). Sensitive periods in human development: Evidence from musical training. Cortex, 47, 1126–1137.CrossRef Penhune, V. B. (2011). Sensitive periods in human development: Evidence from musical training. Cortex, 47, 1126–1137.CrossRef
go back to reference Pfordresher, P. Q. (2005). Auditory feedback in music performance: The role of melodic structure and musical skill. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 1331–1345.PubMed Pfordresher, P. Q. (2005). Auditory feedback in music performance: The role of melodic structure and musical skill. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 1331–1345.PubMed
go back to reference Pfordresher, P. Q. (2012). Musical training and the role of auditory feedback during performance. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1252, 171–178.CrossRef Pfordresher, P. Q. (2012). Musical training and the role of auditory feedback during performance. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1252, 171–178.CrossRef
go back to reference Pfordresher, P. Q. (2019). Sound and action in music performance. San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Pfordresher, P. Q. (2019). Sound and action in music performance. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
go back to reference Pfordresher, P. Q., & Chow, K. (2019). A cost of musical training? Sensorimotor flexibility in musical sequence learning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 26, 967–973.CrossRef Pfordresher, P. Q., & Chow, K. (2019). A cost of musical training? Sensorimotor flexibility in musical sequence learning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 26, 967–973.CrossRef
go back to reference Poggio, T., & Bizzi, E. (2004). Generalization in vision and motor control. Nature, 431, 768–774.CrossRef Poggio, T., & Bizzi, E. (2004). Generalization in vision and motor control. Nature, 431, 768–774.CrossRef
go back to reference Prince, J. B., Stevens, C. J., Jones, M. R., & Tillmann, B. (2018). Learning of pitch and time structures in an artificial grammar setting. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 44, 1201–1214.PubMed Prince, J. B., Stevens, C. J., Jones, M. R., & Tillmann, B. (2018). Learning of pitch and time structures in an artificial grammar setting. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 44, 1201–1214.PubMed
go back to reference Rusconi, E., Kwan, B., Giordano, B. L., Umilta, C., & Butterworth, B. (2006). Spatial representation of pitch height: the SMARC effect. Cognition, 99, 113–129.CrossRef Rusconi, E., Kwan, B., Giordano, B. L., Umilta, C., & Butterworth, B. (2006). Spatial representation of pitch height: the SMARC effect. Cognition, 99, 113–129.CrossRef
go back to reference Schmidt, R. A., & Lee, T. D. (1999). Motor control and learning: A behavioral emphasis (3rd ed.). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics. Schmidt, R. A., & Lee, T. D. (1999). Motor control and learning: A behavioral emphasis (3rd ed.). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
go back to reference Schultz, B. G., Stevens, C. J., Keller, P. E., & Tillmann, B. (2013). The implicit learning of metrical and nonmetrical temporal patterns. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66, 360–380.CrossRef Schultz, B. G., Stevens, C. J., Keller, P. E., & Tillmann, B. (2013). The implicit learning of metrical and nonmetrical temporal patterns. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66, 360–380.CrossRef
go back to reference Shibata, K., Sasaki, Y., Bang, J. W., Walsh, E. G., Machizawa, M. G., Tamaki, M., . . . Watanabe, T. (2017). Overlearning hyperstabilizes a skill by rapidly making neurochemical processing inhibitory-dominant. Nature Neuroscience, 20, 470-475. Shibata, K., Sasaki, Y., Bang, J. W., Walsh, E. G., Machizawa, M. G., Tamaki, M., . . . Watanabe, T. (2017). Overlearning hyperstabilizes a skill by rapidly making neurochemical processing inhibitory-dominant. Nature Neuroscience, 20, 470-475.
go back to reference Taylor, J. E., & Witt, J. K. (2015). Listening to music primes space: pianists, but not novices, simulate heard actions. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 79, 175–182.CrossRef Taylor, J. E., & Witt, J. K. (2015). Listening to music primes space: pianists, but not novices, simulate heard actions. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 79, 175–182.CrossRef
go back to reference Tillmann, B., Bharucha, J. J., & Bigand, E. (2000). Implicit learning of tonality: A self-organizing approach. Psychological Review, 107, 885–913.CrossRef Tillmann, B., Bharucha, J. J., & Bigand, E. (2000). Implicit learning of tonality: A self-organizing approach. Psychological Review, 107, 885–913.CrossRef
go back to reference Zatorre, R. J., Chen, J. L., & Penhune, V. B. (2007). When the brain plays music: Auditory-motor interactions in music perception and production. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8, 547–558.CrossRef Zatorre, R. J., Chen, J. L., & Penhune, V. B. (2007). When the brain plays music: Auditory-motor interactions in music perception and production. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8, 547–558.CrossRef
go back to reference Zhang, J. D., Susino, M., McPherson, G. E., & Schubert, E. (2020). The definition of a musician in music psychology: A literature review and the six-year rule. Psychology of Music, 48, 389–409.CrossRef Zhang, J. D., Susino, M., McPherson, G. E., & Schubert, E. (2020). The definition of a musician in music psychology: A literature review and the six-year rule. Psychology of Music, 48, 389–409.CrossRef
go back to reference Finney, S. A. (2001). FTAP: A Linux-based program for tapping and music experiments. Behavior Research Methods Instruments & Computers, 33, 65–72.CrossRef Finney, S. A. (2001). FTAP: A Linux-based program for tapping and music experiments. Behavior Research Methods Instruments & Computers, 33, 65–72.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Generalization of novel sensorimotor associations among pianists and non-pianists: more evidence that musical training effects are constrained
Auteurs
Peter Q. Pfordresher
Chihiro Honda
Emma Greenspon
Karen Chow
Publicatiedatum
02-06-2020
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 5/2021
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-020-01362-9