Abstract
When a single light cue is given in the visual field, our eyes orient towards it with an average latency of 200 ms. If a second cue is presented at or around the time of the response to the first, a secondary eye movement occurs that represents a reorientation to the new target. While studies have shown that eye movement latencies to ‘single-step’ targets may or may not be lengthened with age, secondary eye movements (during ‘double-step’ displacements) are significantly delayed with increasing age. The aim of this study was to investigate whether the postural challenge posed simply by standing (as opposed to sitting) results in significantly longer eye movement latencies in older adults compared to the young. Ten young (<35 years) and 10 older healthy adults (>65 years) participated in the study. They were required to fixate upon a central target and move their eyes in response to 2 types of stimuli: (1) a single-step perturbation of target position either 15° to the right or left and (2) a double-step target displacement incorporating an initial target jump to the right or left by 15°, followed after 200 ms, by a shift of target position to the opposite side (e.g. +15° then −15°). All target displacement conditions were executed in sit and stand positions with the participant at the same distance from the targets. Eye movements were recorded using electro-oculography. Older adults did not show significantly longer eye movement latencies than the younger adults for single-step target displacements, and postural configuration (stand compared to sit) had no effect upon latencies for either group. We categorised double-step trials into those during which the second light changed after or before the onset of the eye shift to the first light. For the former category, young participants showed faster secondary eye shifts to the second light in the standing position, while the older adults did not. For the latter category of double-step trial, young participants showed no significant difference between sit and stand secondary eye movement latencies, but older adults were significantly longer standing compared to sitting. The older adults were significantly longer than the younger adults across both postural conditions, regardless of when the second light change occurred during the eye shift to the first light. We suggest that older adults require greater time and perhaps attentional processes to execute eye movements to unexpected changes in target position when faced with the need to maintain standing balance.






Similar content being viewed by others
References
Abel LA, Troost BT, Dellosso LF (1983) The effects of age on normal saccadic characteristics and their variability. Vis Res 23:33–37
Anastasopoulos D, Ziavra N, Hollands MA, Bronstein AM (2009) Gaze displacement and inter-segmental coordination during large whole body voluntary rotations. Exp Brain Res 193:323–326
Becker W, Jurgens R (1979) An analysis of the saccadic system by means of double step stimuli. Vis Res 19:967–983
Bock O, Beurskens R, Helmich I, Rein R (2013) Prefrontal neural activation during dual-task walking in old-age: an fNIRS study. J Psychophysiol 27:12
Bono F, Oliveri RL, Zappia M, Aguglia U, Puccio G, Quattrone A (1996) Computerized analysis of eye movements as a function of age. Arch Gerontol Geriatr 22:261–269
Carter JE, Obler L, Woodward S, Albert ML (1983) The effect of increasing age on the latency for saccadic eye-movements. J Gerontol 38:318–320
Creasey H, Rapoport SI (1985) The aging human-brain. Ann Neurol 17:2–10
Di Cesare S, Anastasopoulos D, Bringoux L, Lee PY, Naushahi MJ, Bronstein AM (2013) Influence of postural constraints on eye and head latency during voluntary rotations. Vis Res 15:1–5
Duhamel JR, Colby CL, Goldberg ME (1992) The updating of the representation of visual space in parietal cortex by intended eye-movements. Science 255:90–92
Fujita T, Nakamura S, Ohue M, Fujii Y, Miyauchi A, Takagi Y, Tsugeno H (2005) Effect of age on body sway assessed by computerized posturography. J Bone Miner Metab 23:152–156
Gaveau V, Pisella L, Priot A-M, Fukui T, Rossetti Y, Pelisson D, Prablanc C (2014) Automatic online control of motor adjustments in reaching and grasping. Neuropsychologica 55:25–40
Gaymard B, Ploner CJ, Rivaud S, Vermersch AI, Pierrot-Deseilligny C (1998) Cortical control of saccades. Exp Brain Res 123:159–163
Gredeback G, Kochukhova O (2010) Goal anticipation during action observation is influenced by synonymous action capabilities, a puzzling developmental study. Exp Brain Res 202:493–497
Guerraz M, Bronstein AM (2008) Ocular versus extraocular control of posture and equilibrium. Clin Neurophysiol 38:391–398
Hollands MA, Ziavra NV, Bronstein AM (2004) A new paradigm to investigate the roles of head and eye movements in the coordination of whole-body movements. Exp Brain Res 154:261–266
Hotson JR, Steinke GW (1988) Vertical and horizontal saccades in sging and dementia—failure to inhibit anticipatory saccades. Neuroophthalmology 8:267–273
Hu YB, Walker R (2011) The neural basis of parallel saccade programming: an fMRI study. J Cogn Neurosci 23:3669–3680
Hua S, Leonard JA, Hilderley JA, Stapley PJ (2013) Postural configuration does not alter unperturbed or perturbed reach movement kinematics. Exp Brain Res 227:63–78
Huaman AG, Sharpe JA (1993) Vertical saccades in senescence. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 34:2588–2595
Hytonen M, Pyykko I, Aalto H, Starck J (1993) Postural control and age. Acta Otolaryngol 113:119–122
Ibbotson M, Krekelberg B (2011) Visual perception and saccadic eye movements. Curr Opin Neurobiol 21:553–558
Jacobs JV, Horak FB (2007) Cortical control of postural responses. J Neural Transm 114:1339–1348
Klein C, Fischer B, Hartnegg K, Heiss WH, Roth M (2000) Optomotor and neuropsychological performance in old age. Exp Brain Res 135:141–154
Krippendorf K (1970) Estimating the reliability, systematic error and random error of interval data. Educ Psychol Meas 30:61–70
Krippendorf K (2004) Reliability in content analysis: some common misconceptions and recommendations. Hum Commun Res 30:411–433
Legrand A, Mazars KD, Lazzareschi J, Lemoine C, Olivier I, Barra J, Bucci MP (2013) Differing effects of prosaccades and antisaccades on postural stability. Exp Brain Res 227:397–405
Lord SR, Menz HB, Sherrington C (2006) Home environment risk factors for falls in older people and the efficacy of home modifications. Age Ageing 35:55–59
Maylor EA, Wing AM (1996) Age differences in postural stability are increased by additional cognitive demands. J Gerontol 51B:143–154
McPeek RM, Skavenski AA, Nakayama K (2000) Concurrent processing of saccades in visual search. Vis Res 40:2499–2516
Monzani D, Setti G, Marchioni D, Genovese E, Gherpelli C, Presutti L (2005) Repeated visually-guided saccades improves postural control in patients with vestibular disorders. Acta Otorhinolaryngol Ital 25:224–232
Moschner C, Baloh RW (1994) Age-related-changes in visual tracking. J Gerontol 49:M235–M238
Munoz DP, Broughton JR, Goldring JE, Armstrong IT (1998) Age-related performance of human subjects on saccadic eye movement tasks. Exp Brain Res 121:391–400
Owsley C (2011) Aging and vision. Vis Res 51:1610–1622
Pelisson D, Alahyane N, Panouilleres M, Tilikete C (2010) Sensorimotor adaptation of saccadic eye movements. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 34:1103–1120
Reed-Jones JG, Reed-Jones RJ, Hollands MA (2014) Is the size of the useful field of view affected by postural demands associated with standing and stepping? Neurosci Lett 566:27–31
Rhodes BJ, Bullock D, Verwey WB, Averbeck BB, Page MPA (2004) Learning and production of movement sequences: behavioral, neurophysiological, and modeling perspectives. Hum Mov Sci 23:699–746
Rougier P, Garin M (2006) Performing saccadic eye movements modifies postural control organisation. Clin Neurophysiol 36:235–243
Rubichi S, Neri M, Nicoletti R (1999) Age-related slowing of control processes: evidence from a response coordination task. Cortex 35:573–582
Sarlegna FR (2006) Impairment of online control of reaching movements with aging: a double-step study. Neurosci Lett 403:309–314
Scialfa CT, Joffe KM (1997) Age differences in feature and conjunction search: implications for theories of visual search and generalized slowing. Aging Neuropsychol Cogn 4:227–246
Sharpe JA, Sylvester TO (1978) Effect of aging on horizontal smooth pursuit. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 17:465–468
Speers RA, Kuo AD, Horak FB (2002) Contributions of altered sensation and feedback responses to changes in coordination of postural control due to aging. Gait Posture 16:20–30
Spooner JW, Sakala SM, Baloh RW (1980) Effect of aging on eye tracking. Arch Neurol 37:575–576
Tian J, Ying HS, Zee DS (2013) Revisiting corrective saccades: role of visual feedback. Vis Res 89:54–64
Uchida T, Hashimoto M, Suzuki N, Takegami T, Iwase Y (1979) Effects of periodic saccades on the body sway in human subjects. Neurosci Lett 13:253–258
Uchiyama M, Demura S (2008) Low visual acuity is associated with the decrease in postural sway. Tohoku J Exp Med 216:277–285
van Wegen EE, van Emmerik RE, Riccio GE (2002) Postural orientation: age-related changes in variability and time-to-boundary. Hum Mov Sci 21:61–84
Warabi T, Kase M, Kato T (1984) Effect of aging on the accuracy of visually guided saccadic eye-movement. Ann Neurol 16:449–454
Wheeless LL, Boynton RM, Cohen GH (1966) Eye-movement responses to step and pulse-step stimuli. J Opt Soc Am 56:956
Wong AL, Shelhamer M (2012) Using prediction errors to drive saccade adaptation: the implicit double-step task. Exp Brain Res 222:55–64
Yang Q, Kapoula Z, Debay E, Coubard O, Orssaud C, Samson M (2006) Prolongation of latency of horizontal saccades in elderly is distance and task specific. Vis Res 46:751–759
Young WR, Hollands MA (2012) Newly acquired fear of falling leads to altered eye movement patterns and reduced stepping safety: a case study. PLoS ONE 7:e49765
Zar JH (1999) Biostatistical analysis, 4th edn. Prentice Hall, Upper Saddle River
Acknowledgments
This study received funding from a University of Wollongong, Faculty of Science, Medicine and Health, Small Grant to PJS. We thank Mr. Peter Ihnat (School of Electrical Engineering, University of Wollongong) for the design and manufacture of the electro-oculography system.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Jimenez, S., Hollands, M., Palmisano, S. et al. Age-related effects of increasing postural challenge on eye movement onset latencies to visual targets. Exp Brain Res 234, 1599–1609 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-016-4564-1
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-016-4564-1