Abstract
In the flash-lag illusion, a brief visual flash and a moving object presented at the same location appear to be offset with the flash trailing the moving object. A considerable amount of studies investigated the visual flash-lag effect, and flash-lag-like effects have also been observed in audition, and cross-modally between vision and audition. In the present study, we investigate whether a similar effect can also be observed when using only haptic stimuli. A fast vibration (or buzz, lasting less than 20 ms) was applied to the moving finger of the observers and employed as a “haptic flash.” Participants performed a two-alternative forced-choice (2AFC) task where they had to judge whether the moving finger was located to the right or to the left of the stationary finger at the time of the buzz. We used two different movement velocities (Slow and Fast conditions). We found that the moving finger was systematically misperceived to be ahead of the stationary finger when the two were physically aligned. This result can be interpreted as a purely haptic analogue of the flash-lag effect, which we refer to as “buzz-lag effect.” The buzz-lag effect can be well accounted for by the temporal-sampling explanation of flash-lag-like effects.



Similar content being viewed by others
References
Abend W, Bizzi E, Morasso P (1982) Human arm trajectory formation. Brain 105:331–348
Alais D, Burr D (2003) The “flash-lag” effect occurs in audition and cross-modally. Curr Biol 13:59–63
Alais D, Burr D (2004) The ventriloquist effect results from near-optimal bimodal integration. Curr Biol 14:257–262
Arrighi R, Alais D, Burr D (2005) Neural latencies do not explain the auditory and audio-visual flash-lag effect. Vis Res 45:2917–2925
Baldo MV, Klein SA (1995) Extrapolation or attention shift? Nature 378:565–566
Brenner E, Smeets JB (2000) Motion extrapolation is not responsible for the flash-lag effect. Vis Res 40:1645–1648
Brenner E, van Beers ER, Rotman G, Smeets JB (2006) The role of uncertainty in the systematic spatial mislocalization of moving objects. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 32:811–825
Burr D (1980) Motion smear. Nature 284:164–165
Cavanagh P (1997) Predicting the present. Nature 386:19–21
Dassonville P (1995) Haptic localization and the internal representation of the hand in space. Exp Brain Res 106:434–448
Eagleman DM, Sejnowski TJ (2000) Motion integration and postdiction in visual awareness. Science 287:2036–2038
Hubbard TL (2014) The flash-lag effect and related mislocalizations: findings, properties, and theories. Psychol Bull 140:308–338
Ichikawa M, Masakura Y (2006) Manual control of the visual stimulus reduces the flash-lag effect. Vis Res 46:2192–2203
Ichikawa M, Masakura Y (2010) Reduction of the flash-lag effect in terms of active observation. Atten Percept Psychophys 72:1032–1044
Jones SAH, Fiehler K, Henriques DYP (2012) A task-dependent effect of memory and hand-target on proprioceptive localization. Neuropsychologia 50:1462–1470
Kawato M (1999) Internal models for motor control and trajectory planning. Curr Opin Neurobiol 9:718–727
Keetels M, Vroomen J (2008) Tactile-visual temporal ventriloquism: no effect of spatial disparity. Percept Psychophys 70:765–771
Kirschfeld K, Kammer T (1999) The Fröhlich effect: a consequence of the interaction of visual focal attention and metacontrast. Vis Res 39:3702–3703
Krekelberg B, Lappe M (1999) Temporal recruitment along the trajectory of moving objects and the perception of position. Vis Res 39:2669–2679
Krekelberg B, Lappe M (2000) A model of the perceived relative positions of moving objects based upon a slow averaging process. Vis Res 40:201–215
Lee TC, Khuu SK, Li W, Hayes A (2008) Distortion in perceived image size accompanies flash-lag in depth. J Vis 8:20
Lopez-Moliner J, Linares D (2006) The flash-lag is reduced when the flash is perceived as a sensory consequence of our action. Vis Res 46:2122–2129
Lopez-Moliner J, Smeets JB, Brenner E (2003a) Comparing the sensitivity of manual pursuit and perceptual judgments to pictorial depth effects. Psychol Sci 14:232–236
Lopez-Moliner J, Smeets JB, Brenner E (2003b) Similar effects of a motion-in-depth illusion on manual tracking and perceptual judgements. Exp Brain Res 151:553–556
Mackay DM (1958) Perceptual stability of a stroboscopically lit visual field containing self-luminous objects. Nature 181:507–508
Maij F, de Grave DD, Brenner E, Smeets JB (2011) Misjudging where you felt a light switch in a dark room. Exp Brain Res 213:223–227
Maij F, Wing AM, Medendorp WP (2013) Spatiotemporal integration for tactile localization during arm movements: a probabilistic approach. J Neurophysiol 110:2661–2669
Mateeff S, Hohnsbein J (1988) Perceptual latencies are shorter for motion towards the fovea than for motion away. Vis Res 28:711–719
Metzger W (1932) Versuch einer gemeinamen Theorie der Phänomene Fröhlichs und Hazeloffs und Kritik ihrer Verfahren zur Messung der Empfindungszeit [An attempt toward a common theory of the phenomena of Fröhlich and Hazelhoff and a criticism of their methods to measure sensation time]. Psychologische Forschung 16:176–200
Murakami I (2001) A flash-lag effect in random motion. Vis Res 41:3101–3119
Nieman D, Nijhawan R, Khurana B, Shimojo S (2006) Cyclopean flash-lag illusion. Vis Res 46:3909–3914
Nijhawan R (1994) Motion extrapolation in catching. Nature 370:256–257
Nijhawan R (1997) Visual decomposition of colour through motion extrapolation. Nature 386:66–69
Nijhawan R, Kirschfeld K (2003) Analogous mechanisms compensate for neural delays in the sensory and the motor pathways; evidence from motor flash-lag. Curr Biol 13:749–753
Purushothaman G, Patel SS, Badell HE, Ogmen H (1998) Moving ahead through differential visual latency. Nature 396:424
Rao RP, Eagleman DM, Sejnowski TJ (2001) Optimal smoothing in visual motion perception. Neural Comput 13:1243–1253
Schlag J, Schlag-Rey M (2013) Through the eye, slowly; delays and localization errors in the visual system. Nat Rev Neurosci 3:191–199
Scocchia L, Grosso RA, de’Sperati C, Stucchi N, Baud-Bovy G (2009) Observer’s control of the moving stimulus increases the flash-lag effect. Vis Res 49:2363–2370
Vroomen J, de Gelder B (2004) Temporal ventriloquism: sound modulates the flash-lag effect. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 30:513–518
Watanabe J, Nakatani M, Ando H, Tachi S (2009) Haptic localizations for onset and offset of vibro-tactile stimuli are dissociated. Exp Brain Res 193:483–489
Whitney D, Murakami I, Cavanagh P (2000) Illusory spatial offset of a flash relative to a moving stimulus is caused by differential latencies for moving and flashed stimuli. Vis Res 40:137–149
Wichmann FA, Hill NJ (2001) The psychometric function: I. Fitting, sampling, and goodness of fit. Percept Psychophys 63:1293–1313
Wolpert DM, Ghahramani Z (2000) Computational principles of movement neuroscience. Nat Neurosci 3:1212–1217
Acknowledgments
The research leading to these results has received funding from the European Community’s Seventh Framework Programme FP7/2007–2013 under Grant Agreement Number 214728-2.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Cellini, C., Scocchia, L. & Drewing, K. The buzz-lag effect. Exp Brain Res 234, 2849–2857 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-016-4687-4
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-016-4687-4