Abstract
This study compared the association of adolescent delinquency with that of their best friend and remaining social network. Findings are reported from The National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health, a multi-wave nationally representative panel study of adolescents who were in grades 7–12 in 1994. Four delinquent outcomes were examined: Smoking, getting drunk, fighting, and a variety index of general delinquency. All analyses were replicated for three distinct criteria for identifying a “best friend.” We also examined several moderating factors and potential interrelationships between the best friend and remaining friendship group. Relative to the influence of the best friend, the influence of the remaining friendship group increased with group size, and with larger absolute disparities in delinquency levels between best and remaining friends. Our findings extend knowledge on the influence of best friends, and further underscore the importance of whether peer behaviors are measured directly (from the peers themselves) or indirectly (when focal respondents estimate the delinquent behavior of their peers).
Similar content being viewed by others
Notes
The literature currently lacks an accepted definition of “best friend.” Instead, there are several credible alternative criteria. An individual’s best friend may simply be the person he or she perceives to be his or her best friend. Or, that person’s best friend may also be the individual he or she spends the most time with, does the widest range of activities with, or feels emotionally closest to. These criteria may not identify the same “best friend.” For these reasons, the ensuing analyses are replicated for three different “best friend” criteria.
Also relevant is the emerging structural perspective on peer influence (Haynie 2001, 2002; Krohn et al. 1988). Rather than specific connections between individuals, this perspective emphasizes the structural characteristics of peer networks and individuals’ positions within them as important determinants of the behavioral concordance among network members. For example, Haynie (2001) found that the density of a friendship network (proportion of actual ties to possible ties between members) and an adolescent’s centrality and popularity within that network all affect the degree to which network members behave similarly to one another. Rather than compete with the perspectives above, the structural approach complements them by identifying additional determinants of peer influence those perspectives do not tend to address. Under the structural perspective, peer influence cannot be fully understood without specific attention to features of groups and individuals’ positions within them.
For example, in the Add health study, the in-school survey was given to all students in each school.
This criterion excluded 146 additional respondents who were either 10, 11, or 19 years old at the in-school interview. These respondents were clearly non-normative for grades 7–12, with no counterbalancing possibility of grade normative representation at these ages.
We used Stata’s -ice- command to impute (see Royston 2004, 2005a, 2005b) the missing values of the following variables: respondent in-school delinquency (general, smoking, drinking, and fighting), public assistance, school attachment, parental education, respondent GPA, parental attachment, and two parents at home. Missing values of the dependent variables and key explanatory variables were not imputed. All analyses included dummy variables which indicated whether or not the value of an independent variable was imputed.
The complexities of the Add Health data also necessitate appropriately accounting for missing data in order to accurately calculate point estimates and to ensure generalizability of results. All analyses followed the recommendations of Chantala and Tabor (1999, p. 11).
This roster also included names from sister schools associated with the respondent’s school.
The Add Health data have two other options for identifying friends. First is the receive network, which consists of all individuals who named the respondent as a friend. Second, the nomination and receive network consists of all individuals who either the respondent named as a friend or who named the respondent as a friend. Since the results were fundamentally the same under all three approaches to constructing an egocentric network, the ensuing analyses use the nomination network.
The Wave II and in-school general delinquency indices do not match precisely but prior research using the same outcomes measured at Wave I has shown that the in-school minor delinquency index is a reasonable and conservative predictor of personal/peer delinquency association when the outcome is the more comprehensive Wave I general delinquency index (Haynie 2001, see also Payne and Cornwell 2007).
Recall that the findings were comparable for the other two criteria for identifying best friends.
Cases with valid responses for both the mother and father were assigned the average of the two.
In each case, Poisson regression was inappropriate because the overdispersion parameter was statistically distinguishable from zero.
F = 5.58, df = (1, 109), p = 0.019.
Several p-values rose above .05 in part because we split the sample roughly in half and lost some statistical power.
References
Akers RL (1985) Deviant behavior: a social learning approach. Wadsworth, Belmont
Akers RL, Jensen GF (2006) The empirical status of social learning theory of crime and deviance: the past, present, and future. In: Cullen FT, Wright JP, Blevins KR (eds) Taking stock: the status of criminological theory. Transaction Publishers, New Brunswick, pp 37–76
Ary DV, Biglan A (1988) Longitudinal changes in adolescent cigarette smoking behavior: onset and cessation. J Behav Med 11:361–382
Baerveldt C, Van Rossem R, de Verman M, Weerman F (2004) Student’s delinquency and correlates with strong and weaker ties: a study of students’ networks in Dutch high schools. Connections 26:11–28
Bauman K, Carver K, Gleiter K (2001) Trends in parent and friend influence during adolescence: the case of adolescent cigarette smoking. Addict Behav 26:349–361
Brendgen Mara, Vitaro Frank, Bukowski WM (2000) Deviant friends and early adolescents’ emotional and behavioral adjustment. J Res Adolesc 10:173–189
Burgess R, Akers RC (1966) A differential association-reinforcement theory of criminal behavior. Soc Probl 14:336–383
Chantala K, Tabor J (1999) Strategies to perform a design-based analysis using the add health data.http://www.cpc.unc.edu/projects/addhealth/pubs/guides
Chassin L, Presson CC, Sherman SJ, Montello D, McGrew J (1986) Changes in peer and parent influence during adolescence: longitudinal versus cross-sectional perspectives on smoking initiation. Dev Psychol 22:327–334
DeRosier ME, Cillessen AHN, Coie JD, Dodge KA (1994) Group social context and children’s aggressive behavior. Child Dev 65:1068–1079
Elliott DS, Menard S (1996) Delinquent friends and delinquent behavior: temporal and developmental patterns. In: Hawkins DJ (ed) Delinquency and crime current theories. Cambridge University Press, New York, pp 28–67
Elliott DS, Huizinga D, Ageton SS (1985) Explaining delinquency and drug use. Sage, Beverly Hills
Ennett ST, Bauman KE (1994) The contribution of influence and selection to adolescent peer group homogeneity: the case of adolescent cigarette smoking. J Pers Soc Psychol 67:653–663
Festinger L, Hoffman PJ, Lawrence DH (1954) A theory of social comparison processes. Hum Relat 7:117–140
Fuligni AJ, Eccles JS, Barber BL (1995) The long-term effects of seventh-grade ability grouping in mathematics. J Early Adolesc 15:58–89
Gifford-Smith M, Dodge KA, Dishion TJ, McCord J (2005) Peer influence in children and adolescents: crossing the bridge from development to intervention science. J Abnorm Child Psychol 33:255–265
Giordano PC (1995) The wider circle of friends in adolescence. Am J Sociol 101:661–697
Glueck S, Glueck E (1950) Unraveling juvenile delinquency. Harvard University Press, Cambridge
Gottfredson M, Hirschi T (1990) A general theory of crime. Stanford University Press, Stanford
Granovetter MS (1973) The strength of weak ties. Am J Sociol 78:1360–1380
Hallinan MT (1979) Structural effects of children’s friendships and cliques. Soc Psychol Q 42:43–54
Hartup WH (1993) Adolescents and their friends. New Dir Child Dev 60:3–22
Haynie DL (2001) Delinquent peers revisited: does network structure matter? Am J Sociol 106:1013–1057
Haynie DL (2002) Friendship networks and delinquency: the relative nature of peer delinquency. J Quant Criminol 18:99–134
Haynie DL, Osgood DW (2005) Reconsidering peers and delinquency: how do peers matter? Soc Forces 84:1109–1130
Hirschi T (1969) Causes of delinquency. University of California Press, Berkeley
Howes C (1989) Peer interaction of young children. Monographs of the society for research in child development. 53 (Serial No 217)
Hussong A (2002) Differentiating peer contexts and risk for adolescent substance abuse. J Youth Adolesc 31:207–220
Iannotti RJ, Bush PJ, Weinfurt KP (1996) Perceptions of friends’ use of alcohol, cigarettes, and marijuana: a longitudinal analysis. Addict Behav 21:615–632
Jaccard J, Blanton H, Dodge T (2005) Peer influences on risk behavior: an analysis of the effects of a close friend. Dev Psychol 41:135–147
Jang SJ (1999) Age-varying effects of family, school, and peers on delinquency: a multilevel modeling test of interactional theory. Criminology 37:643–685
Jussim L, Osgood DW (1989) Influence and similarity among friends: an integrative model applied to incarcerated adolescents. Soc Psychol Q 52:98–112
Kandel DB (1978) Homophily, selection, and socialization in adolescent friendships. Am J Sociol 84:427–436
Korn EL, Graubard BI (1990) Simultaneous testing of regression coefficients with complex survey data: use of bonferroni t statistics. Am Stat 44:270–276
Krohn MD, Massey J, Zielinski M (1988) Role overlap, network multiplexity, and adolescent deviant behavior. Soc Psychol Q 51(4):346–356
Krohn MD, Lizotte AJ, Thornberry TP, Smith C, McDowall D (1996) Reciprocal causal relationships among drug use, peers and beliefs: a five-wave panel model. J Drug Issues 26:405–428
Laird RD, Petite GS, Dodge KA, Bates JE (1999) Best friendships, group relationships, and antisocial behavior in early adolescence. J Early Adolesc 19:413–437
Loeber R, Stouthamer-Loeber M (1986) Family factors as correlates and predictors of juvenile conduct problems and delinquency. In: Tonry M, Morris N (eds) Crime and justice, vol 7. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 29–149
Maguin E, Loeber R (1996) Academic performance and delinquency. Crime And Justice: A Rev Res 20:145–264
Matsueda R, Anderson K (1998) The dynamics of delinquent peers and delinquent behavior. Criminology 36:269–308
McGloin JM (2009) Delinquency balance: revisiting peer influence. Criminology 47:439–477
McGloin JM, Piquero A (2010) On the relationship between co-offending network redundancy and offending versatility. J Res Crime Delinq 47:63–90
McGloin JM, Piquero A, Sullivan C, Bacon S (2008) Investigating stability of co-offending and co-offenders among a sample of youthful offenders. Criminology 46:155–188
Morrison GM, D’Incau B (1997) The web of zero-tolerance: characteristics of students who are recommended for expulsion from school. Educ Treat Children 20:316–335
Nagin D, Paternoster R (2000) Population heterogeneity and state dependence: state of the evidence and directions for future research. J Quant Criminol 16:117–144
Newcomb AF, Bagwell CL (1995) Children’s friendship relations: a meta-analytic review. Psychol Bull 117:306–347
Park KA (1992) Preschoolers reactions to loss of a best friend: developmental trends and individual differences. Child Study J 22:233–252
Paternoster R, Brame R, Mazerolle P, Piquero A (1998) Using the correct statistical test for the equality of regression coefficients. Criminology 36(4):859–866
Payne D, Cornwell B (2007) Reconsidering peer influences on delinquency: do less proximate contacts matter? J Quant Criminol 23:127–149
Prinstein MJ (2007) Assessment of adolescents’ preference–and reputation–based peer status using sociometric experts. Merrill Palmer Q 53:243–261
Royston P (2004) Multiple imputation of missing values. Stata J 4:227–241
Royston P (2005a) Multiple imputation of missing values: update. Stata J 5:188–201
Royston P (2005b) Multiple Imputation of missing Values: update of ice”. Stata J 5:527–536
Sarnecki J (2001) Delinquent networks. Youth co-offending in stockholm Cambridge studies in criminology. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge
Sebanc AM, Kearns KT, Hernandez MD, Galvin KB (2007) Predicting having a best friend in young children: individual characteristics and friendship features. J Genet Psychol 168:81–96
Selfhout M, Branje S, Meeus W (2008) The development of delinquency and perceived friendship quality in adolescent best friendship dyads. J Abnorm Child Psychol 36:471–485
Stafford M, Warr M (1993) A reconceptualization of general and specific deterrence. J Res Crime Delinq 30:123–135
Sutherland EH (1947) Criminology. JB, Lippincott
Thornberry TP, Krohn MD (1997) Peers, drug use, and delinquency. In: Stoff DM, Breiling J, Maser JD (eds) Handbook of antisocial behavior. Wiley, New York, pp 218–233
Thornberry TP, Lizotte AJ, Krohn MD, Farnworth M, Jang SJ (1991) Testing interactional theory: an examination of reciprocal causal relationships among family, school and delinquency. J Crim Law Criminol 82:3–35
Thornberry TP, Lizotte AJ, Krohn MD, Farnworth M, Jang SJ (1994) Delinquent peers, beliefs, and delinquent behavior: a longitudinal test of interactional theory. Criminology 32:1–47
Tolson J, Urberg KA (1993) Similarity among adolescent best friends. J Adolesc Res 8:274–288
Urberg K (1992) Locus of peer influence: social crowd and best friend. J Youth Adolesc 21:439–450
Urberg KA, Shyu SJ, Liang J (1990) Peer influence in adolescent cigarette smoking. Addict Behav 15:247–255
Urberg K, Degirmencioglu SM, Pilgrim C (1997) Close friend and group influence on adolescent cigarette smoking and alcohol use. Dev Psychol 33:834–844
Warr M (1993) Age, peers, and delinquency. Soc Forces 72:247–264
Warr M (2002) Companions in crime: the social aspects of criminal conduct. Cambridge University Press, New York
Weerman F, Smeenk WH (2005) Peer similarity in delinquency for different types of friends: a comparison using two measurement models. Criminology 43:499–524
Wilson JQ, Herrnstein RJ (1985) Crime and human nature. Simon and Schuster, New York
Zhang L, Messner SF (2000) The effects of alternative measures of delinquent peers on self-reported delinquency. J Res Crime Delinq 37:324–337
Acknowledgment
This research uses data from Add Health, a program project directed by Kathleen Mullan Harris and designed by J. Richard Udry, Peter S. Bearman, and Kathleen Mullan Harris at the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, and funded by grant P01-HD31921 from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, with cooperative funding from 23 other federal agencies and foundations. Special acknowledgment is due Ronald R. Rindfuss and Barbara Entwisle for assistance in the original design. Information on how to obtain the Add Health data files is available on the Add Health website (http://www.cpc.unc.edu/addhealth). No direct support was received from grant P01-HD31921 for this analysis.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Appendix
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Rees, C., Pogarsky, G. One Bad Apple May Not Spoil the Whole Bunch: Best Friends and Adolescent Delinquency. J Quant Criminol 27, 197–223 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10940-010-9103-9
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10940-010-9103-9