Abstract
Tasks requiring the subject to tap in synchrony to a regular sequence of stimulus events (e.g., clicks) usually elicit a response pattern in which the tap precedes the click by about 30-50 msec. This “negative asynchrony” was examined, first, by instructing subjects to use different effectors for tapping (hand vs. foot; Experiments 1 and 2), and second, by administering extrinsic auditory feedback in addition to the intrinsic tactile/kinesthetic feedback (Experiment 2). Experiment 3 controlled whether the results observed in Experiment 2 were due to purely sensory factors within the auditory modality. Results suggest that taps are synchronized with clicks at the central level by superimposing two sensory codes in time: the tactile/kinesthetic code that represents the tap (the afferent movement code) and the auditory code that represents the click (the afferent code that results from the guiding signal). Because the processing times involved in code generation are different for these two central codes, the tap has to lead over the click.
Article PDF
Similar content being viewed by others
Avoid common mistakes on your manuscript.
References
Allport, A., MacKay, D. G., Prinz, W., &Scheerer, E. (Eds.) (1987).Language perception and production. London: Academic Press.
Aschersleben, G. (1994).Afferente Informationen und die Synchronisation von Ereignissen [Afferent information and the synchronization of events]. Frankfurt: Lang.
Auxiette, C. (1992).Coordination et synchronisation de deux sequences sonores perçues et produites [The coordination and synchronization of two perceived and produced sound sequences]. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Paris.
Bard, C., Paillard, J., Lajoie, Y., Fleury, M., Teasdale, N., Forget, R., &Lamarre, Y. (1992). Role of the afferent information in the timing of motor commands: A comparative study with a deafferent patient.Neuropsychologia,30, 201–206.
Bard, C., Paillard, J., Teasdale, N., Fleury, M., &Lajoie, Y. (1991). Self-induced versus reactive triggering of synchronous hand and heel movement in young and old subjects. In J. Requin & G. E. Stelmach (Eds.),Tutorials in motor neuroscience (pp. 189–196). Amsterdam: Kluwer.
Collyer, C. E., Broadbent, H. A., &Church, R. M. (1992). Categorical time production: Evidence for discrete timing in motor control.Perception & Psychophysics,51, 134–144.
Collyer, C. E., Broadbent, H. A., &Church, R. M. (1994). Preferred rates of repetitive tapping and categorical time production.Perception & Psychophysics,55, 443–453.
Dennett, D. C., &Kinsbourne, M. (1992). Time and the observer: The where and the when of consciousness and the brain.Behavioral & Brain Sciences,15, 183–247.
Dunlap, K. (1910). Reactions on rhythmic stimuli, with attempt to synchronize.Psychological Review,17, 399–416.
Elliott, L. L. (1971). Backward and forward masking.Audiology,10, 65–76.
Fraisse, P. (1966). L’anticipation de stimulus rythmiques, vitesse d’établissement et précision de la synchronisation [Anticipation of rhythmical stimuli, set-up speed and accuracy of synchronization].L’Année Psychologique,66, 15–36.
Fraisse, P. (1980). Les synchronisations sensori-motrices aux rythmes [The sensorimotor synchronization of rhythms]. In J. Requin (Ed.),Anticipation et comportement (pp. 233–257). Paris: Centre National.
Fraisse, P., Oléron, G., &Paillard, J. (1958). Sur les repères sensoriels qui permettent de contrôler les mouvements d’accompagnement de stimuli périodiques [On the sensory reference points that allow for controlling movements accompanying periodic stimuli].L’Année Psychologique,58, 322–338.
Franek, M., Radil, T., Indra, M., &Lánsky, P. (1987). Following complex rhythmical acoustical patterns by tapping.International Journal of Psychophysiology,5, 187–192.
Geisser, S., &Greenhouse, S. W. (1958). An extension of Box’s results on the use of F-distribution in multivariate analysis.Annals of Mathematical Statistics,29, 885–891.
Gérard, C., &Auxiette, C. (1992). The processing of musical prosody by musical and nonmusical children.Music Perception,10, 93–126.
Greenwald, A. G. (1970). Sensory feedback mechanisms in performance control: With special reference to the ideo-motor mechanism.Psychological Review,77, 73–99.
Hary, D., &Moore, G. P. (1985). Temporal tracking and synchronization strategies.Human Neurobiology,4, 73–77.
Hary, D., &Moore, G. P. (1987). Synchronizing human movement with an external clock source.Biological Cybernetics,56, 305–311.
Hawkins, H. L., Thomas, G., Presson, J., Cozic, A., &Brookmire, D. (1974). Precategorical selective attention and tonal specificity in auditory recognition.Journal of Experimental Psychology,103, 530–538.
Hirsh, I. J., &Sherrick, C. E. (1961). Perceived order in different sense modalities.Journal of Experimental Psychology,62, 423–432.
Ilmberger, J., Müller, U., Pöppel, E., Mates, J., &Radil, T. (1990). Timing of rhythmic tapping and lateralization.Activas Nervosa Superior,32, 142.
Ivry, R. B., Keele, S. W., &Diener, H. C. (1988). Dissociation of the lateral and medial cerebellum in movement timing and movement execution.Experimental Brain Research,73, 167–180.
Kelso, J. A. S., &Kay, B. A. (1987). Information and control: A macroscopic analysis of perception-action coupling. In H. Heuer & A. F. Sanders (Eds.),Perspectives on perception and action (pp. 3–32). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Koch, R. (1992).Sensumotorische Synchronisation: Eine Kostenanalyse [Sensorimotor synchronization: A cost analysis] (Paper 11/92). Munich: Max-Planck-Institute for Psychological Research.
Kolers, P. A., &Brewster, J. M. (1985). Rhythms and responses.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,11, 150–167.
Lynn, G. K., &Small, A. M. (1977). Interactions of backward and forward masking.Journal of the Acoustical Society of America,61, 185–189.
Marcus, S. M. (1976).Perceptual centres. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Cambridge University.
Massaro, D. W. (1970). Preperceptual auditory images.Journal of Experimental Psychology,85, 411–417.
Massaro, D. W. (1990). An information-processing analysis of perception and action. In O. Neumann & W. Prinz (Eds.),Relationships between perception and action: Current approaches (pp. 133–166). Berlin: Springer-Verlag.
Massaro, D. W., &Idson, W. L. (1978). Target-mask similarity in backward recognition masking of perceived tone duration.Perception & Psychophysics,24, 225–236.
Mates, J. (1991). Extending the model of self-paced periodic responding: Comment on Vos and Ellermann (1989).Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,17, 877–879.
Mates, J. (1994a). A model of synchronization of motor acts to a stimulus sequence: I. Timing and error corrections.Biological Cybernetics,70, 463–473.
Mates, J. (1994b). A model of synchronization of motor acts to a stimulus sequence: II. Stability analysis, error estimation and simulations.Biological Cybernetics,70, 475–484.
Mates, J., Radil, T., &Pöppel, E. (1992). Cooperative tapping: Time control under different feedback conditions.Perception & Psychophysics,52, 691–704.
Meltzoff, A. N., Kuhl, P. K., &Moore, M. K. (1991). Perception, representation, and the control of action in newborns and young infants: Towards a new synthesis. In M. J. Weiss & P. R. Zelazo (Eds.),Newborn attention: Biological constraints and the influence of experience (pp. 377–411). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Morton, J., Marcus, S. M., &Frankish, C. (1976). Perceptual centers (P-centers).Psychological Review,83, 405–408.
Najenson, T., Ron, S., &Behroozi, K. (1989). Temporal characteristics of tapping responses in healthy subjects and in patients who sustained cerebrovascular accident.Brain, Behavior & Evolution,33, 175–178.
Oléron, G. (1961). Attitude et utilisation des repères dans la synchronisation de stimuli périodiques [The role and the use of reference points in the synchronization of periodic stimuli].L’Année Psychologique,61, 59–78.
Paillard, J. (1949). Quelques données psychophysiologiques relatives au déclenchement de la commande motrice [Some psychophysiological data relating to the triggering of motor commands].L’Année Psychologique,48, 28–47.
Pastore, R. E., Harris, L. B., &Goldstein, L. (1980). Auditory forward and backward masking interaction.Perception & Psychophysics,28, 547–549.
Pattee, H. H. (1974). Discrete and continuous processes in computers and brains. In M. Conrad, W. Guttinger, & M. D. Cin (Eds.),Physics and mathematics of the nervous system (pp. 128–148). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Peters, M. (1989). The relationship between variability of intertap intervals and interval duration.Psychological Research,51, 38–42.
Pöppel, E., Müller, U., & Mates, J. (1990, October).Temporal constraints in synchronization of motor responses to a regular sequence of stimuli. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Society for Neuroscience, St. Louis.
Povel, D. J. (1981). Internal representation of simple temporal patterns.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,7, 3–18.
Prinz, W. (1990). A common coding approach to perception and action. In O. Neumann & W. Prinz (Eds.),Relationships between perception and action: Current approaches (pp. 167–201). Berlin: Springer-Verlag.
Prinz, W. (1992). Why don’t we perceive our brain states?European Journal of Cognitive Psychology,4, 1–20.
Rutschmann, J., &Link, R. (1964). Perception of temporal order of stimuli differing in sense mode and simple reaction time.Perceptual & Motor Skills,18, 345–352.
Sanders, A. F. (1980). Stage analysis of reaction processes. In G. E. Stelmach & J. Requin (Eds.),Tutorials in motor behavior (pp. 331–354). Amsterdam: North-Holland.
Schulze, H.-H. (1992). The error correction model for the tracking of a random metronome: Statistical properties and an empirical test. In F. Macar, V. Pouthas, & W. J. Friedman (Eds.),Time, action and cognition (pp. 275–286). Dordrecht: Kluwer.
Schütte, H. (1978a). Ein Funktionsschema für die Wahrnehmung eines gleichmäßigen Rhythmus in Schallimpulsen [A functional scheme for perceiving a regular rhythm in auditory impulses].Biological Cybernetics,29, 49–55.
Schütte, H. (1978b). Subjektiv gleichmäßiger Rhythmus: Ein Beitrag zur zeitlichen Wahrnehmung von Schallereignissen [Subjective regular rhythm: A contribution to the temporal perception of auditory events].Acustica,41, 197–206.
Shibasaki, H., Barrett, G., Halliday, E., &Halliday, A. M. (1981). Cortical potentials associated with voluntary foot movement in man.Electroencephalography & Clinical Neurophysiology,52, 507–516.
Sternberg, S., &Knoll, R. L. (1973). The perception of temporal order: Fundamental issues and a general model. In S. Kornblum (Ed.),Attention and performance IV (pp. 629–685). New York: Academic Press.
Stevens, L. T. (1886). On the time sense.Mind,11, 393–404.
Stone, S. A. (1926). Prior entry in the auditory-tactual complication.American Journal of Psychology,37, 284–287.
Summers, J. J., Bell, R., &Burns, B. D. (1989). Perceptual and motor factors in the imitation of simple temporal patterns.Psychological Research,50, 23–27.
Terhardt, E., &Schütte, H. (1976). Akustische Rhythmus-Wahrnehmung: Subjektive Gleichmäßigkeit [Acoustical rhythm perception: Subjective regularity].Acustica,35, 122–126.
Truman, G., &Hammond, G. R. (1990). Temporal regularity of tapping by the left and right hands in timed and untimed finger tapping.Journal of Motor Behavior,22, 521–535.
Vorberg, D., &Hambuch, R. (1978). On the temporal control of rhythmic performance. In J. Requin (Ed.),Attention and performance VII (pp. 535–555). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Vorberg, D., &Hambuch, R. (1984). Timing of two-handed rhythmic performance. In J. Gibbon & L. Allan (Eds.),Timing and time perception (Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, Vol. 423, pp. 390–406). New York: New York Academy of Sciences.
Vorberg, D., &Wing, A. (1993). Modelle für die Variabilität und Abhängigkeit bei der zeitlichen Steuerung [Modeling variability and dependence in timing]. In H. Heuer & S. Keele (Eds.),Enzyklopädie der Psychologie, Serie Kognition: Band III. Psychomotorik (pp. 223–320). Göttingen: Hogrefe.
Vos, P. G., &Ellermann, H. H. (1989). Precision and accuracy in the reproduction of simple tone sequences.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,15, 179–187.
Vos, P. G., &Helsper, E. L. (1992). Tracking simple rhythms: On-beat versus off-beat performance. In F. Macar, V. Pouthas, & W. J. Friedman (Eds.),Time, action and cognition (pp. 287–299). Dordrecht: Kluwer.
Vos, P. G., Helsper, E. L., &van Krysbergen, N. (1992). Tracking simple rhythms under pseudo-synchronization conditions. InProceedings of the Fourth Rhythm Workshop: Rhythm Perception and Production (pp. 37–42). Bourges: CNRS.
Welford, A. T. (1968).Fundamentals of skill. London: Methuen.
Welford, A. T. (1980).Reaction times. London: Academic Press.
Wing, A. M. (1977). Perturbations of auditory feedback delay and the timing of movement.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,3, 175–186.
Wing, A. M. (1982). Timing and co-ordination of repetitive bimanual movements.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,37A, 339–348.
Wing, A. M., &Kristofferson, A. B. (1973a). Response delays and the timing of discrete motor responses.Perception & Psychophysics,14, 5–12.
Wing, A. M., &Kristofferson, A. B. (1973b). The timing of interresponse intervals.Perception & Psychophysics,13, 455–460.
Woodrow, H. (1932). The effect of rate of sequence upon the accuracy of synchronization.Journal of Experimental Psychology,15, 357–379.
Yamanishi, J., Kawato, M., &Suzuki, R. (1979). Studies on human finger tapping neural networks by phase transition curves.Biological Cybernetics,33, 199–208.
Yamanishi, J., Kawato, M., &Suzuki, R. (1980). Two coupled oscillators as a model for the coordinated finger tapping by both hands.Biological Cybernetics,37, 219–225.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Aschersleben, G., Prinz, W. Synchronizing actions with events: The role of sensory information. Perception & Psychophysics 57, 305–317 (1995). https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03213056
Received:
Accepted:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03213056