Skip to main content

Welkom bij THIM Hogeschool voor Fysiotherapie & Bohn Stafleu van Loghum

THIM Hogeschool voor Fysiotherapie heeft ervoor gezorgd dat je Mijn BSL eenvoudig en snel kunt raadplegen. Je kunt je links eenvoudig registreren. Met deze gegevens kun je thuis, of waar ook ter wereld toegang krijgen tot Mijn BSL. Heb je een vraag, neem dan contact op met helpdesk@thim.nl.

Registreer

Om ook buiten de locaties van THIM, thuis bijvoorbeeld, van Mijn BSL gebruik te kunnen maken, moet je jezelf eenmalig registreren. Dit kan alleen vanaf een computer op een van de locaties van THIM.

Eenmaal geregistreerd kun je thuis of waar ook ter wereld onbeperkt toegang krijgen tot Mijn BSL.

Login

Als u al geregistreerd bent, hoeft u alleen maar in te loggen om onbeperkt toegang te krijgen tot Mijn BSL.

Top
Gepubliceerd in:

26-11-2016 | Original Article

Modulating adaptation to emotional faces by spatial frequency filtering

Auteurs: Giulia Prete, Bruno Laeng, Luca Tommasi

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 2/2018

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

In four experiments, we investigated the presence and strength of perceptual aftereffects to emotional faces, using spatial frequency filtering to manipulate awareness of emotional content. We presented angry and happy faces as adapters and used a control condition without adaptation. Participants were subsequently requested to judge the friendliness level of a neutral target face. We confirmed the well-known aftereffect for unfiltered emotional faces in Experiment 1. In the experiment, friendliness judgments were greater for the angry than the happy or the control condition. In Experiment 2, in which the “hidden” emotional expression contained in the low spatial frequencies (LSF) was superimposed to the neutral expression presented in the rest of SF of the same image (emotional hybrid faces), the difference in friendliness judgments between angry and happy was significant, but none of the two conditions significantly differed from the control. In Experiment 3, faces were presented at LSF, confirmed a difference between the two emotions, but only the judgments of angry faces differed from the control condition. In Experiment 4, we extended the initial finding to stimuli presented at middle and high spatial frequencies (M-HSF). Finally, a comparison among all experiments revealed that the aftereffect was stronger with angry faces filtered at M-HSF than all of the other filtering conditions, whereas there were no differences for happy faces. We conclude that spatial frequency filtering influences aftereffects and that these effects are also related to emotional awareness. The results are discussed with reference to the dual route model of visual perception.
Literatuur
go back to reference Adams, W. J., Gray, K. L., Garner, M., & Graf, E. W. (2010). High-level face adaptation without awareness. Psychological Science, 21(2), 205–210.CrossRefPubMed Adams, W. J., Gray, K. L., Garner, M., & Graf, E. W. (2010). High-level face adaptation without awareness. Psychological Science, 21(2), 205–210.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Blakemore, C., & Campbell, F. W. (1969). On the existence of neurones in the human visual system selectively sensitive to the orientation and size of retinal images. Journal of Physiology, 203, 237–260.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Blakemore, C., & Campbell, F. W. (1969). On the existence of neurones in the human visual system selectively sensitive to the orientation and size of retinal images. Journal of Physiology, 203, 237–260.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Butler, A., Oruc, I., Fox, C. J., & Barton, J. J. (2008). Factors contributing to the adaptation aftereffects of facial expression. Brain Research, 1191, 116–126.CrossRefPubMed Butler, A., Oruc, I., Fox, C. J., & Barton, J. J. (2008). Factors contributing to the adaptation aftereffects of facial expression. Brain Research, 1191, 116–126.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Chen, J., Yang, H., Wang, A., & Fang, F. (2010). Perceptual consequences of face viewpoint adaptation: Face viewpoint aftereffect, changes of differential sensitivity to face view, and their relationship. Journal of Vision, 10(3), 1–11.CrossRef Chen, J., Yang, H., Wang, A., & Fang, F. (2010). Perceptual consequences of face viewpoint adaptation: Face viewpoint aftereffect, changes of differential sensitivity to face view, and their relationship. Journal of Vision, 10(3), 1–11.CrossRef
go back to reference Costen, N. P., Parker, D. M., & Craw, I. (1996). Effect of highpass and lowpass spatial filtering on face identification. Perception and Psychophysics, 58, 602–612.CrossRefPubMed Costen, N. P., Parker, D. M., & Craw, I. (1996). Effect of highpass and lowpass spatial filtering on face identification. Perception and Psychophysics, 58, 602–612.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference D’Ascenzo, S., Tommasi, L., & Laeng, B. (2014). Imagining sex and adapting to it: Different aftereffects after perceiving versus imagining faces. Vision Research, 96, 45–52.CrossRefPubMed D’Ascenzo, S., Tommasi, L., & Laeng, B. (2014). Imagining sex and adapting to it: Different aftereffects after perceiving versus imagining faces. Vision Research, 96, 45–52.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Eastwood, J. D., Smilek, D., & Merikle, P. M. (2001). Differential attentional guidance by unattended faces expressing positive and negative emotion. Perception and Psychophysics, 63(6), 1004–1013.CrossRefPubMed Eastwood, J. D., Smilek, D., & Merikle, P. M. (2001). Differential attentional guidance by unattended faces expressing positive and negative emotion. Perception and Psychophysics, 63(6), 1004–1013.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Fox, C. J., & Barton, J. J. S. (2007). What is adapted in face adaptation? The neural representations of expression in the human visual system. Brain Research, 1127, 80–89.CrossRefPubMed Fox, C. J., & Barton, J. J. S. (2007). What is adapted in face adaptation? The neural representations of expression in the human visual system. Brain Research, 1127, 80–89.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Gold, J., Bennett, P. J., & Sekuler, A. B. (1999). Identification of band-pass filtered letters and faces by human and ideal observers. Vision Research, 39, 3537–3560.CrossRefPubMed Gold, J., Bennett, P. J., & Sekuler, A. B. (1999). Identification of band-pass filtered letters and faces by human and ideal observers. Vision Research, 39, 3537–3560.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Goolsby, B. A., Grabowecky, M., & Suzuki, S. (2005). Adaptive modulation of color salience contingent upon global form coding and task relevance. Vision Research, 45(7), 901–930.CrossRefPubMed Goolsby, B. A., Grabowecky, M., & Suzuki, S. (2005). Adaptive modulation of color salience contingent upon global form coding and task relevance. Vision Research, 45(7), 901–930.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Graham, N. V. S. (1989). Visual pattern analyzers. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRef Graham, N. V. S. (1989). Visual pattern analyzers. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Jiang, Y., & He, S. (2006). Cortical responses to invisible faces: Dissociating subsystems for facial-information processing. Current Biology, 16(20), 2023–2029.CrossRefPubMed Jiang, Y., & He, S. (2006). Cortical responses to invisible faces: Dissociating subsystems for facial-information processing. Current Biology, 16(20), 2023–2029.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Khalid, S., Ansorge, U., & Finkbeiner, M. (2015). Supraliminal but no subliminal priming by high-spatial frequency faces in a face-sex discrimination task. Psychology, 6(12), 1486.CrossRef Khalid, S., Ansorge, U., & Finkbeiner, M. (2015). Supraliminal but no subliminal priming by high-spatial frequency faces in a face-sex discrimination task. Psychology, 6(12), 1486.CrossRef
go back to reference Laeng, B., Profeti, I., Saether, L., Adolfsdottir, S., Lundervold, A. J., Vangberg, T., … Waterloo, K. (2010). Invisible expressions evoke core impressions. Emotion, 10(4), 573–586. doi:10.1037/a0018689.CrossRefPubMed Laeng, B., Profeti, I., Saether, L., Adolfsdottir, S., Lundervold, A. J., Vangberg, T., … Waterloo, K. (2010). Invisible expressions evoke core impressions. Emotion, 10(4), 573–586. doi:10.​1037/​a0018689.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Laeng, B., Saether, L., Holmlund, T., Wang, C. E. A., Waterloo, K., Eisemann, M., & Halvorsen, M. (2013). Invisible emotional expressions influence social judgments and papillary responses of both depressed and non-depressed individuals. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 1–7. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00291.CrossRef Laeng, B., Saether, L., Holmlund, T., Wang, C. E. A., Waterloo, K., Eisemann, M., & Halvorsen, M. (2013). Invisible emotional expressions influence social judgments and papillary responses of both depressed and non-depressed individuals. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 1–7. doi:10.​3389/​fpsyg.​2013.​00291.CrossRef
go back to reference LeDoux, J. (1996). The emotional brain: The mysterious underpinnings of emotional life. New York: Touchstone Press. LeDoux, J. (1996). The emotional brain: The mysterious underpinnings of emotional life. New York: Touchstone Press.
go back to reference Leknes, S., Wessberg, J., Ellingsen, D. M., Chelnokova, O., Olausson, H., & Laeng, B. (2013). Oxytocin enhances pupil dilation and sensitivity to ‘hidden’ emotional expressions. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8(7), 741–749. doi:10.1093/scan/nss062.CrossRefPubMed Leknes, S., Wessberg, J., Ellingsen, D. M., Chelnokova, O., Olausson, H., & Laeng, B. (2013). Oxytocin enhances pupil dilation and sensitivity to ‘hidden’ emotional expressions. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8(7), 741–749. doi:10.​1093/​scan/​nss062.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Leopold, D. A., O’Toole, A. J., Vetter, T., & Blanz, V. (2001). Prototype-referenced shape encoding revealed by high-level aftereffects. Nature Neuroscience, 4(1), 89–94.CrossRefPubMed Leopold, D. A., O’Toole, A. J., Vetter, T., & Blanz, V. (2001). Prototype-referenced shape encoding revealed by high-level aftereffects. Nature Neuroscience, 4(1), 89–94.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Liddell, B. J., Brown, K. J., Kemp, A. H., Barton, M. J., Das, P., Peduto, A., & Williams, L. M. (2005). A direct brainstem–amygdala–cortical ‘alarm’ system for subliminal signals of fear. Neuroimage, 24(1), 235–243.CrossRefPubMed Liddell, B. J., Brown, K. J., Kemp, A. H., Barton, M. J., Das, P., Peduto, A., & Williams, L. M. (2005). A direct brainstem–amygdala–cortical ‘alarm’ system for subliminal signals of fear. Neuroimage, 24(1), 235–243.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Livingstone, M., & Hubel, D. (1988). Segregation of form, color, movement, and depth: Anatomy, physiology, and perception. Science, 240, 740–749.CrossRefPubMed Livingstone, M., & Hubel, D. (1988). Segregation of form, color, movement, and depth: Anatomy, physiology, and perception. Science, 240, 740–749.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Lundqvist, D., Flykt, A., & Öhman, A. (1998). The Karolinska directed emotional faces (KDEF). Stockholm: Department of Neurosciences Karolinska Hospital. Lundqvist, D., Flykt, A., & Öhman, A. (1998). The Karolinska directed emotional faces (KDEF). Stockholm: Department of Neurosciences Karolinska Hospital.
go back to reference Morris, J. S., Öhman, A., & Dolan, R. J. (1999). A subcortical pathway to the right amygdala mediating “unseen” fear. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 96(4), 1680–1685. doi:10.1073/pnas.96.4.1680.CrossRef Morris, J. S., Öhman, A., & Dolan, R. J. (1999). A subcortical pathway to the right amygdala mediating “unseen” fear. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 96(4), 1680–1685. doi:10.​1073/​pnas.​96.​4.​1680.CrossRef
go back to reference Moutoussis, K. (2015). The physiology and psychophysics of the color-form relationship: A review. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1407.PubMedPubMedCentral Moutoussis, K. (2015). The physiology and psychophysics of the color-form relationship: A review. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1407.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Murphy, S. T., & Zajonc, R. B. (1993). Affect, cognition, and awareness: affective priming with optimal and suboptimal stimulus exposures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64(5), 723. Murphy, S. T., & Zajonc, R. B. (1993). Affect, cognition, and awareness: affective priming with optimal and suboptimal stimulus exposures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64(5), 723.
go back to reference Nasanen, R. (1999). The spatial frequency bandwidth used in the recognition of facial images. Vision Research, 39, 3824–3833.CrossRefPubMed Nasanen, R. (1999). The spatial frequency bandwidth used in the recognition of facial images. Vision Research, 39, 3824–3833.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh Inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97–114.CrossRefPubMed Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh Inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97–114.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Palumbo, R., Laeng, B., & Tommasi, L. (2013). Gender-specific aftereffects following adaptation to silhouettes of human bodies. Visual Cognition, 21(1), 1–12.CrossRef Palumbo, R., Laeng, B., & Tommasi, L. (2013). Gender-specific aftereffects following adaptation to silhouettes of human bodies. Visual Cognition, 21(1), 1–12.CrossRef
go back to reference Pasley, B. N., Mayes, L. C., & Schultz, R. T. (2004). Subcortical discrimination of unperceived objects during binocular rivalry. Neuron, 42(1), 163–172.CrossRefPubMed Pasley, B. N., Mayes, L. C., & Schultz, R. T. (2004). Subcortical discrimination of unperceived objects during binocular rivalry. Neuron, 42(1), 163–172.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Pessoa, L., & Adolphs, R. (2010). Emotion processing and the amygdala: From a ‘low road’ to ‘many roads’ of evaluating biological significance. Nature Review Neuroscience, 11(11), 773–783. doi:10.1038/nrn2920.CrossRef Pessoa, L., & Adolphs, R. (2010). Emotion processing and the amygdala: From a ‘low road’ to ‘many roads’ of evaluating biological significance. Nature Review Neuroscience, 11(11), 773–783. doi:10.​1038/​nrn2920.CrossRef
go back to reference Pessoa, L., McKenna, M., Gutierrez, E., & Ungerleider, L. G. (2002). Neural processing of emotional faces requires attention. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 99(17), 11458–11463.CrossRef Pessoa, L., McKenna, M., Gutierrez, E., & Ungerleider, L. G. (2002). Neural processing of emotional faces requires attention. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 99(17), 11458–11463.CrossRef
go back to reference Prete, G., Capotosto, P., Zappasodi, F., Laeng, B., & Tommasi, L. (2015a). The cerebral correlates of subliminal emotions: An eleoencephalographic study with emotional hybrid faces. European Journal of Neuroscience, 42(11), 2952–2962. doi:10.1111/ejn.13078.CrossRefPubMed Prete, G., Capotosto, P., Zappasodi, F., Laeng, B., & Tommasi, L. (2015a). The cerebral correlates of subliminal emotions: An eleoencephalographic study with emotional hybrid faces. European Journal of Neuroscience, 42(11), 2952–2962. doi:10.​1111/​ejn.​13078.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Prete, G., D’Ascenzo, S., Laeng, B., Fabri, M., Foschi, N., & Tommasi, L. (2015b). Conscious and unconscious processing of facial expressions: Evidence from two split-brain patients. Journal of Neuropsychology, 9, 45–63. doi:10.1111/jnp.12034.CrossRefPubMed Prete, G., D’Ascenzo, S., Laeng, B., Fabri, M., Foschi, N., & Tommasi, L. (2015b). Conscious and unconscious processing of facial expressions: Evidence from two split-brain patients. Journal of Neuropsychology, 9, 45–63. doi:10.​1111/​jnp.​12034.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Rhodes, G., Jeffery, L., Watson, T. L., Jaquet, E., Winkler, C., & Clifford, C. W. G. (2004). Orientation-contingent face aftereffects and implications for face-coding mechanisms. Current Biology, 14, 2119–2123.CrossRefPubMed Rhodes, G., Jeffery, L., Watson, T. L., Jaquet, E., Winkler, C., & Clifford, C. W. G. (2004). Orientation-contingent face aftereffects and implications for face-coding mechanisms. Current Biology, 14, 2119–2123.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Rotshtein, P., Vuilleumier, P., Winston, J., Driver, J., & Dolan, R. (2007). Distinct and convergent visual processing of high and low spatial frequency information in faces. Cerebral Cortex, 17(11), 2713–2724.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Rotshtein, P., Vuilleumier, P., Winston, J., Driver, J., & Dolan, R. (2007). Distinct and convergent visual processing of high and low spatial frequency information in faces. Cerebral Cortex, 17(11), 2713–2724.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Schiller, P. H., & Malpeli, J. G. (1977). Properties and tectal projections of monkey retinal ganglion cells. Journal of Neurophysiology, 40, 428–445.CrossRefPubMed Schiller, P. H., & Malpeli, J. G. (1977). Properties and tectal projections of monkey retinal ganglion cells. Journal of Neurophysiology, 40, 428–445.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Schyns, P. G., & Oliva, A. (1994). From blobs to boundary edges: Evidence for time-and spatial-scale-dependent scene recognition. Psychological Science, 5(4), 195–200.CrossRef Schyns, P. G., & Oliva, A. (1994). From blobs to boundary edges: Evidence for time-and spatial-scale-dependent scene recognition. Psychological Science, 5(4), 195–200.CrossRef
go back to reference Sperandio, I., Lak, A., & Goodale, M. A. (2012). Afterimage size is modulated by size-contrast illusions. Journal of Vision, 12(2), 18.CrossRefPubMed Sperandio, I., Lak, A., & Goodale, M. A. (2012). Afterimage size is modulated by size-contrast illusions. Journal of Vision, 12(2), 18.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Tamietto, M., & De Gelder, B. (2010). Neural bases of the non-conscious perception of emotional signals. Nature Review Neuroscience, 11(10), 697–709. doi:10.1038/nrn2889.CrossRef Tamietto, M., & De Gelder, B. (2010). Neural bases of the non-conscious perception of emotional signals. Nature Review Neuroscience, 11(10), 697–709. doi:10.​1038/​nrn2889.CrossRef
go back to reference Taylor, S. E. (1991). Asymmetrical effects of positive and negative events: The mobilization-minimization hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 110(1), 67.CrossRefPubMed Taylor, S. E. (1991). Asymmetrical effects of positive and negative events: The mobilization-minimization hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 110(1), 67.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Varga, E. T., Elif, K., Antal, A., Zimmer, M., Harza, I., Paulus, W., & Gyula, K. (2007). Cathodal transcranial direct current stimulation over the parietal cortex modifies facial gender adaptation. Ideggyogy Sz, 60(11–12), 474–479.PubMed Varga, E. T., Elif, K., Antal, A., Zimmer, M., Harza, I., Paulus, W., & Gyula, K. (2007). Cathodal transcranial direct current stimulation over the parietal cortex modifies facial gender adaptation. Ideggyogy Sz, 60(11–12), 474–479.PubMed
go back to reference Vuilleumier, P., Armony, J. L., Driver, J., & Dolan, R. J. (2003). Distinct spatial frequency sensitivities for processing faces and emotional expressions. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 624–631. doi:10.1038/nn1057.CrossRefPubMed Vuilleumier, P., Armony, J. L., Driver, J., & Dolan, R. J. (2003). Distinct spatial frequency sensitivities for processing faces and emotional expressions. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 624–631. doi:10.​1038/​nn1057.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Williams, M. A., Morris, A. P., McGlone, F., Abbott, D. F., & Mattingley, J. B. (2004). Amygdala responses to fearful and happy facial expressions under conditions of binocular suppression. The Journal of Neuroscience, 24(12), 2898–2904.CrossRefPubMed Williams, M. A., Morris, A. P., McGlone, F., Abbott, D. F., & Mattingley, J. B. (2004). Amygdala responses to fearful and happy facial expressions under conditions of binocular suppression. The Journal of Neuroscience, 24(12), 2898–2904.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Wincenciak, J., Dzhelyova, M., Perrett, D. I., & Barraclough, N. E. (2013). Adaptation to facial trustworthiness is different in female and male observers. Vision Research, 87, 30–34.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Wincenciak, J., Dzhelyova, M., Perrett, D. I., & Barraclough, N. E. (2013). Adaptation to facial trustworthiness is different in female and male observers. Vision Research, 87, 30–34.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Yamashita, J. A., Hardy, J. L., De Valois, K. K., & Webster, M. A. (2005). Stimulus selectivity of figural aftereffects for faces. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31(3), 420.PubMed Yamashita, J. A., Hardy, J. L., De Valois, K. K., & Webster, M. A. (2005). Stimulus selectivity of figural aftereffects for faces. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31(3), 420.PubMed
go back to reference Yang, E., Hong, S. W., & Blake, R. (2010). Adaptation aftereffects to facial expressions suppressed from visual awareness. Journal of Vision, 10(12), 24.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Yang, E., Hong, S. W., & Blake, R. (2010). Adaptation aftereffects to facial expressions suppressed from visual awareness. Journal of Vision, 10(12), 24.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Yip, A. W., & Sinha, P. (2002). Contribution of color to face recognition. Perception, 31, 995–1003.CrossRefPubMed Yip, A. W., & Sinha, P. (2002). Contribution of color to face recognition. Perception, 31, 995–1003.CrossRefPubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
Modulating adaptation to emotional faces by spatial frequency filtering
Auteurs
Giulia Prete
Bruno Laeng
Luca Tommasi
Publicatiedatum
26-11-2016
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 2/2018
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-016-0830-x