Introduction
Adolescents with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) experience challenges with social interaction and communication, affecting their ability to develop and maintain friendships (Reichow & Volkmar,
2010). One challenge during adolescence is the increased emphasis on peer relationships, and the expected decreased reliance on parents. For adolescents with ASD, this may be a particularly difficult period, marked by negative social outcomes, such as fewer friends, lack of social support, increased peer rejection, and limited social engagement (Shattuck et al.,
2011).
The complexity of conversational skills, in particular, is an area of difficulty for autistic adolescents,
1 in part due to underlying deficits in social communication, social awareness, social motivation, and social cognition (Carter et al.,
2005; Chevallier et al.,
2012). Various theories have been proposed to understand the nature of these difficulties, including the weak central coherence theory (Happe & Frith, 2006) and the systemized processing bias (Baron-Cohen et al.,
2003). The weak central coherence theory poses that autistic individuals have a cognitive perceptual deficit in global information processing, or “seeing the big picture”, which may relate to difficulties with integrating social information. The systemized processing bias suggests that rather than having weak overall global processing, autistic individuals have a higher ability to predict the behavior of a system (systemizing) than the behavior of a person (empathizing), which in effect, leads to social difficulties and withdrawal. Correspondingly, adolescents with ASD are typically less involved in social activities and thus have fewer opportunities to practice social interactions in comparison to neurotypical youth, limiting their conversational opportunities (Chevallier et al.,
2012; Shattuck et al.,
2011). These conversational challenges and reduced social interactions make developing and maintaining friendships difficult for adolescents with ASD (Laugeson & Frankel,
2010).
Despite the social challenges experienced by youth on the spectrum, there is ample evidence suggesting a desire for social involvement and the development of social relationships. For example, Church et al. (
2000) found that autistic adolescents in middle school with average to above average cognitive abilities expressed some interest in interacting with their peers. It is not unusual for these adolescents to report concerns over making friends or not having any friends (Locke et al.,
2010). Taken together, the evidence suggests that adolescents with ASD report poorer quality friendships, greater loneliness, and greater social anxiety than their neurotypical peers (Bauminger & Kasari,
2000).
Adolescents with ASD with average to high cognitive abilities may initiate more social interactions compared to their peers with lower cognitive abilities; however, their higher cognitive awareness and recognition of their own lack of friends may actually increase their likelihood of loneliness (Bauminger et al.,
2003), peer rejection (Church et al.,
2000), and bullying (Zeedyk et al.,
2014). Given the reported challenges with developing and maintaining friendships, social skills have been a primary target for intervention, especially among adolescents with ASD with cognitive abilities in the average range. With an increase in the prevalence of ASD in the last few decades, there has also been an increase in research evaluating the effectiveness of group-based social skills interventions (GSSIs; Gates et al.,
2017). One of the most well-studied GSSIs for adolescents with ASD is the Program for the Education and Enrichment of Relational Skills (PEERS; Laugeson et al.,
2009,
2012). However, research on GSSIs, including PEERS, has been limited by methodological issues, such as limited objective outcome measures and a lack of focus on skill generalization (Wolstencroft et al.,
2018; Zheng et al.,
2021).
Though the PEERS intervention has a strong literature base relative to other social skills interventions for adolescents with ASD, most of the reported treatment gains have come from the participants themselves. Informants have their own biases, attributions, and expectations that clearly influence their responses (Stratis & Lecavalier,
2015). Self-report measures are commonly used in the assessment of youth with social challenges, but numerous research studies have suggested that self-report of adolescents with ASD should be interpreted with caution (Stratis & Lecavalier,
2015). One such study found that children and adolescents with ASD reported greater levels of social skills and social competence, compared to parent reports of the same constructs (Knott et al.,
2006). The use of multiple informants in the assessment of psychological functioning is considered a “gold standard” in the assessment of psychopathology in children and adolescents (e.g., Mash & Hunsley,
2005). However, due to their inherent bias, questionnaire measures should be combined with other more objective measures of treatment outcomes, such as behavioral observations, cognitive or neuropsychological measures, and sociometric tasks (Gates et al.,
2017; Kaat & Lecavalier, 2014). Observations of behavioral change by blind raters may be one of the most objective measures of treatment outcome, but they are used less frequently than questionnaire measures (Kaat & Lecavalier, 2014). For example, only two of the eight RCTs conducted on the PEERS for Adolescents intervention have utilized observational measures (Rabin et al.,
2018; Yoo et al.,
2014).
An observational measure that has shown promise as an ecologically valid treatment outcome measure following PEERS is the Contextual Assessment of Social Skills (CASS; Dolan et al.,
2016; Rabin et al.,
2018; Ratto et al.,
2011; White et al.,
2015). The CASS is a semi-structured live role-play measure of conversational skills consisting of a three-minute filmed conversation between the adolescent and an unfamiliar peer (i.e., a research confederate). Simmons et al. (
2020) evaluated the utility and validity of the CASS as a measure of social cognition and social behavior for adolescents with ASD, finding the CASS to have strong internal and external validity. Results of this study suggest that the CASS should be used as part of a multimethod battery for assessing outcomes of clinical interventions in individuals with ASD.
In addition to a lack of objective forms of assessment, there is a lack of reported diversity in GSSI studies. Though cross-cultural replication studies with PEERS have been conducted with Asian and European samples (e.g., Rabin et al.,
2018; Shum et al.,
2018; Yamada et al.,
2020; Yoo et al.,
2014), Latinx families, especially those who are socioeconomically disadvantaged, have been consistently underrepresented in ASD intervention research (Bernal & Domenech Rodríguez,
2009; Pickard et al.,
2019; Ratto et al.,
2017). Previous research on adapting ASD interventions for Latinx samples indicates that culturally sensitive adaptations are necessary for successful intervention implementation (Chlebowski et al.,
2020; Huey & Polo,
2008; Ratto et al.,
2017). Thus, one objective of this study as a whole was to examine whether Latinx families achieve similar benefits after participation in PEERS. Finally, though waitlist and no-treatment control groups have been employed in many PEERS studies, no studies have utilized a typically developing, no-treatment control group to evaluate whether improvements in social competence among ASD participants are clinically meaningful and based on the intervention itself (Zheng et al.,
2021).
While the focus of this study is evaluating a social skills intervention for autistic teens, it is important to acknowledge that the neurodiversity paradigm has shifted the focus of autism intervention research away from the neurodivergent individual and towards society (e.g., Jurgens,
2020). In line with the neurodiversity paradigm, the social model of autism rejects the view that autism is a disability and instead emphasizes a strengths-based perspective in which interventions are focused on improving autistic individuals’ capabilities and well-being, rather than correcting their perceived ‘deficits’. It should therefore be noted that many of the measures of social functioning used in this study were created based on the assumption that various skills should be judged with neurotypical individuals as the ‘standard’ (e.g., measuring social skills using the framework that neurotypical social behavior is ‘correct’ and that autistic individuals can ‘improve’ their social skills). Despite these inherent limitations, the measures used in the current study have been validated across many studies and continue to be widely used to assess social functioning in autistic individuals. Thus, when we refer to “improvements” in social functioning or “higher” or “lower” social skills, we are doing so in the context of the norms of these measures.
Procedure
For adolescents participating in PEERS, eligibility was initially assessed during a phone screening interview with the parent using the Phone Screening Script (Laugeson & Frankel,
2010). Adolescents’ motivation to participate was assessed during a brief phone call with the adolescent, and again during an intake appointment using the Teen Mental Status Checklist from the manual. Prior to participating in
treatment, all families came in individually to the university autism center for an intake appointment. During this appointment, informed parental consent and adolescent assent were obtained, and the adolescents were administered the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule, 2nd edition (ADOS-2; Lord et al.,
2012) to confirm autism eligibility, and the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence, 2nd edition (WASI-II; Wechsler,
2011) to determine that their IQ was 70 or above. Adolescents and parents completed various questionnaires, including a demographic form and measures of adolescent social functioning, parent acculturation and language, and parent and family impact. Adolescents also participated in a three-minute conversational interaction with an unfamiliar peer (i.e., the CASS).
Within two weeks following the 16-week treatment, adolescents and parents came in for a post-appointment and completed all of the same measures from the intake appointment, excluding the diagnostic and cognitive assessments, demographic questionnaire, and Teen Mental Status Checklist. Four months after the completion of the intervention, families came in for a follow-up appointment, which was identical to the post-appointment, in order to assess maintenance of treatment gains. The second cohort of adolescents and parents completed their follow-up measures online via Qualtrics due to in-person COVID-19 restrictions. With the exception of the CASS, TD participants completed the same measures in person at three timepoints, each four months apart, to assess the stability of scores over time; for three TD participants, follow-up measures were completed via Qualtrics due to COVID-19 restrictions. To increase retention rates, families were each compensated $110, spread out throughout the pre, post, and follow-up appointments.
Treatment
The PEERS Curriculum for School-Based Professionals (Laugeson,
2014) comprised the intervention. It was administered in a 16-week format, and was used concurrently with the original PEERS Treatment Manual (Laugeson & Frankel,
2010) for the parent portion of the intervention. Adolescents and parents attended 90-minute concurrent but separate sessions. Treatment was conducted by two PEERS Certified Providers, and all procedures were overseen by a licensed psychologist. Behavioral coaches who were undergraduate or graduate students assisted with role-play demonstrations, behavior management, attendance and homework tracking, and tracking treatment fidelity.
For teens, treatment sessions used didactic instruction in a small group format, which included role-play demonstrations, behavioral rehearsal activities with reinforcement and corrective feedback, and weekly homework assignments related to social engagement (Ellingsen et al.,
2017). To promote generalization of the skills outside of the clinic setting, parents were taught how to become social coaches for their teens by using key words taught by the program when providing feedback or practicing skills at home, and identifying appropriate extracurricular activities that can serve as a source of friends for their teens (Ellingsen et al.,
2017; Laugeson et al.,
2009,
2012). Teens received points for completing homework assignments and for participation during the didactic lesson and behavioral rehearsal exercises. See Table
2 for detailed information regarding program participation and treatment compliance.
Table 2
Program participation and treatment compliance for ASD participants (n = 13)
Teen attendance | 14.9 | 1.4 | 12—16 |
Parent attendance | 15.0 | 1.0 | 13—16 |
Teen-reported homework completion | 66.3 | 12.8 | 41.2—84.3 |
Parent-reported homework completion | 66.0 | 13.8 | 41.2—89.2 |
Teen total points earned | 256.3 | 91.4 | 165—469 |
Topics of instruction included using appropriate conversational skills; choosing appropriate friends; using electronic communication appropriately and safely; using humor appropriately; initiating, joining, and exiting conversations with peers; organizing successful get-togethers; being a good sport when playing games/sports with peers; handling arguments and disagreements; handling rejection, teasing, bullying, rumors/gossip and cyber bullying; and changing a bad reputation (Laugeson,
2014). Treatment fidelity was assessed using a checklist. Behavioral coaches were responsible for ensuring that the group leader covered each component of the intervention in the treatment manual. 100% treatment fidelity was reported in both the adolescent and parent groups.
Spanish Translation
In an effort to adapt the PEERS intervention for Latinx families, materials provided to families were professionally translated into Spanish prior to the start of the program, including the parent handouts and homework assignment sheets, the program welcome letter, the planned absence sheet, and the graduation flyer. The adolescent groups were conducted in English, and the parent groups were conducted simultaneously in English and Spanish by a bilingual group leader. As many of the parents recruited for this study were bilingual, a combination of English and Spanish in the parent group was determined to be the most culturally sensitive and inclusive format, with additional supports as needed to maintain participation comfort (e.g., a one-on-one translator for parents with lower levels of English comprehension).
Results
Results addressing impact of the PEERS intervention on social functioning as reported by the adolescent participants will be presented first, followed by the analyses examining parent reports and observed conversational skills.
Adolescent Self-Report Measures
To evaluate the impact of the PEERS intervention on adolescent social skills knowledge, the total TASSK score was examined; a significant group by time interaction was observed (
F(2,42) = 38.72,
p < 0.001,
ηp2 = 0.65). A Bonferroni post-hoc test showed that social knowledge improved significantly from pre- to post- treatment for ASD participants (
p < 0.001), and was stable from post-treatment to follow up (
p = 0.54). TD teens not participating in
treatment had stable scores on the TASSK across time-points (
p’s > 0.05). At pre-treatment, ASD and TD participants had similar levels of social knowledge on the TASSK (
p = 0.88). However, at post-treatment and follow-up, ASD participants had greater PEERS-specific social knowledge than did TD participants (
p’s < 0.001).
In terms of adolescent-reported social engagement (QSQ), the group by time interaction was not significant for hosted get-togethers (F(2,42) = 0.02, p = 0.98, \(\eta_p^{2}\) = 0.001) or invited get-togethers (F(2,42) = 0.20, p = 0.82, \(\eta_p^{2}\) = 0.009). However, to account for the potential effects of the COVID-19 pandemic on get-togethers, a two-way within-subjects repeated measures ANOVA was conducted for the ASD group, demonstrating that teens with ASD reported hosting significantly more get-togethers from pre- to post- intervention (F(1,12) = 5.82, p = 0.03, \(\eta_p^{2}\) = 0.33). In contrast, there were no significant changes in the frequency of invited get-togethers from pre- to post- treatment for adolescents with ASD (p > 0.05). TD adolescents reported a stable frequency of QSQ hosted and invited get-togethers across time (all p’s > 0.05). Notably, TD participants reported significantly more hosted get-togethers than ASD participants at pre-treatment (p = 0.001) and follow-up (p = 0.04); however, between-group differences were not significant at post-treatment (p = 0.28). Similarly, between-group differences in invited get-togethers were significant at pre-treatment (p = 0.04), but were not significant at post-treatment or follow-up (p’s > 0.05). Note that follow-up data were collected during the COVID-19 stay-at-home order in California, during which get-togethers were highly discouraged.
Since follow-up data on anxiety were not collected from TD participants, a three-way within-group repeated measures ANOVA was conducted for the ASD group only. There was a marginal effect of time on SIAS social anxiety scores for adolescents with ASD (F(2,24) = 3.31, p = 0.08, \(\eta_p^{2}\) = 0.22). A Bonferroni post-hoc test also demonstrated marginal improvements in social anxiety from pre- to post- intervention for ASD participants (p = 0.05), which were maintained at follow-up (p > 0.05). TD participants had stable SIAS scores from pre- to post-treatment (p = 0.62). There were no significant between-group differences in social anxiety at pre- or post- intervention (p’s > 0.05).
For both ASD and TD participants, loneliness scores on the LSDQ were stable across time. Participants with ASD reported significantly more loneliness on the LSDQ than TD participants across time (all p’s < 0.05). Self-concept scores on the Piers-Harris were also stable across time and no significant effects were observed. Friendship quality scores on the FQS were also stable across time for both groups. Notably, between-group differences in friendship quality were significant at pre-treatment (p = 0.05), but were not significant at post-treatment or follow-up (p’s > 0.05).
Parent-Report Measures
The group by time interaction was not significant for parent-reported social skills on the SSIS. However, for ASD participants, a three-way within subjects repeated measures ANOVA demonstrated a significant main effect of time on SSIS social skills (F(2, 24) = 12.83, p < 0.001, \(\eta_p^{2}\) = 0.52). Bonferroni post-hoc tests showed significant improvements on the SSIS from pre- to post- treatment (p < 0.01), which remained stable at follow up (p > 0.05). TD participants had stable SSIS social skills scores across time (p’s > 0.05), and demonstrated significantly higher overall social skills compared to ASD participants at all time points (all p’s < 0.05). In terms of problem behaviors on the SSIS, the group by time interaction was not significant. Between-group differences were found at all time points, such that parents of ASD participants reported significantly greater problem behaviors than parents of TD participants (all p’s < 0.05). However, there was a marginal effect of time on problem behaviors for ASD participants in the within-subjects three-way repeated measures ANOVA (F(2,24) = 2.79, p = 0.08, \(\eta_p^{2}\) = 0.20). Post-hoc comparisons did not show significant effects; however, a two-way (pre- and post-intervention) ANOVA indicated significant improvements in problem behaviors from pre- to post- treatment (F(1,12) = 5.09, p = 0.04) for ASD participants.
There was a significant group by time interaction on parent-reported social responsiveness, as indicated by the SRS total score (F(2, 44) = 4.22, p = 0.03, \(\eta_p^{2}\) = 0.16). Bonferroni post hoc tests revealed significant improvements from pre- to post- treatment on the SRS-2 for ASD participants (p = 0.008), which were maintained at follow-up (p > 0.05). TD teens had stable SRS-2 scores over time (p’s > 0.05), demonstrating significantly higher social responsiveness than ASD participants at each time point (all p’s < 0.001).
To examine parent-reported social engagement, a within-subjects three-way repeated measures ANOVA was conducted for ASD participants only. There was a significant main effect of time on hosted get-togethers for the ASD group (
F(2, 24) = 8.84,
p = 0.001,
\(\eta_p^{2}\) = 0.42). Bonferroni post hoc tests showed significant improvements from pre- to post- treatment (
p < 0.01), which were maintained at follow-up (
p > 0.05). A pre-post analysis conducted with TD adolescents (due to missing follow-up data) revealed stable scores on parent-reported hosted get-togethers between the first two timepoints (
p > 0.05). Group differences between ASD and TD participants in the frequency of hosted get-togethers were significant at pre-treatment (
p = 0.001), but were not significant at post-treatment (
p = 0.67). For QSQ invited get-togethers, the group by time interaction was not significant, and scores were stable across time for both ASD and TD participants (all
p’s > 0.05). Parents reported a similar frequency of invited get-togethers for ASD and TD participants across all time points (all
p’s > 0.05). See Table
3 for detailed ANOVA results and scores at pre, post, and follow-up for ASD and TD participants.
Table 3
Group (2) by time (3) repeated measures ANOVA for ASD and TD participants
Parent-report measures |
SRS total T-scorea | 4.22* | 0.03 | 0.16 |
ASD | 74.85 (12.84) | 68.85 (15.06) | 68.69 (14.71) | | | |
TD | 44.00 (3.77) | 42.55 (2.77) | 46.00 (5.69) | | | |
SSIS social skillsa | 1.90 | 0.17 | 0.08 |
ASD | 81.62 (19.19) | 87.85 (19.05) | 90.31 (19.84) | | | |
TD | 106.64 (12.34) | 107.09 (11.26) | 108.09 (8.96) | | | |
SSIS prob behaviorsa | 0.89 | 0.40 | 0.04 |
ASD | 120.23 (17.74) | 116.00 (18.51) | 118.50 (18.02) | | | |
TD | 93.82 (7.92) | 94.00 (10.11) | 100.00 (16.89) | | | |
QSQ hostedb | 8.84** | 0.001 | 0.42 |
ASD | 0.23 (0.60) | 2.00 (1.47) | 1.15 (1.46) | | | |
TD | 2.18 (1.66) | 2.45 (3.42) | N/A | | | |
QSQ inviteda | 0.001 | 0.99 | 0.00 |
ASD | 0.69 (1.32) | 1.08 (1.12) | 1.15 (.99) | | | |
TD | 1.64 (1.43) | 2.00 (1.55) | 2.09 (1.45) | | | |
Adolescent Self-Report Measures |
TASSK-R Total | 38.72*** | 0.000 | 0.65 |
ASD | 14.31 (2.62) | 25.00 (3.65) | 24.00 (3.58) | | | |
TD | 14.50 (3.17) | 15.80 (2.86) | 15.10 (3.48) | | | |
LSDQ Total | 0.53 | 0.60 | 0.02 |
ASD | 56.38 (13.07) | 58.46 (10.95) | 60.00 (11.37) | | | |
TD | 69.30 (10.02) | 70.00 (10.37) | 69.20 (5.20) | | | |
SIAS Totala,b | 3.31 | 0.08 | 0.22 |
ASD | 34.85 (18.10) | 27.62 (14.63) | 30.77 (15.93) | | | |
TD | 22.82 (12.81) | 20.91 (14.70) | N/A | | | |
PH2 Total | 0.02 | 0.98 | 0.00 |
ASD | 47.46 (11.00) | 49.08 (5.60) | 47.85 (10.28) | | | |
TD | 52.11 (10.11) | 53.67 (10.82) | 52.00 (9.91) | | | |
FQS Total | 2.16 | 0.13 | 0.11 |
ASD | 84.75 (12.91) | 86.62 (12.19) | 86.69 (13.58) | | | |
TD | 96.25 (10.01) | 93.00 (9.37) | 91.63 (10.00) | | | |
QSQ Hosted | 0.02 | 0.98 | 0.00 |
ASD | 0.54 (.97) | 2.23 (2.52) | 1.15 (1.63) | | | |
TD | 2.80 (1.87) | 4.20 (5.77) | 3.30 (2.95) | | | |
QSQ Invited | 0.20 | 0.82 | 0.01 |
ASD | 0.62 (1.19) | 1.00 (1.23) | 0.77 (.93) | | | |
TD | 2.20 (2.20) | 3.20 (3.99) | 2.30 (2.95) | | | |
Corroboration of Findings from Observational Measure
Contextual Assessment of Social Skills (CASS) observational ratings were analyzed to examine whether perceived improvements in social functioning were corroborated by observed improvements in conversational skills. Though descriptive statistics revealed higher average ratings in several conversational domains from pre- to post- treatment, these improvements were not significant (all
p’s > 0.05). Only two significant differences emerged from pre- to post-treatment: participants initiated significantly fewer topic changes (
p < 0.05) and had greater kinesic arousal (i.e., demonstrated more fidgeting) at post-treatment (
p < 0.05). See Table
4 for comparisons of selected CASS codes at pre- and post- intervention.
Table 4
Comparisons of selected CASS codes at pre- and post- intervention (n = 13)
# of Questions | 3.77 (2.68) | 2.77 (2.46) | 1.93 | .08 |
# of topic changes | 2.69 (1.75) | 1.92 (1.71) | 2.25 | 0.04* |
Vocal expressiveness | 3.77 (1.83) | 3.92 (2.10) | 0.35 | 0.73 |
Gestures | 3.00 (1.96) | 3.92 (2.18) | 1.45 | 0.17 |
Kinesic arousal | 3.92 (1.61) | 2.85 (1.41) | 2.94 | 0.01* |
Overall involvement | 4.46 (1.51) | 4.77 (1.69) | 1.17 | 0.26 |
Overall rapport | 4.23 (1.48) | 4.23 (1.79) | 0.00 | 1.00 |
CASS total score | 15.15 (6.72) | 13.69 (6.70) | −1.45 | 0.17 |
To examine whether observed improvements on the CASS were associated with perceived improvements in social functioning, bivariate Pearson correlations were conducted between the mean difference scores on the two global CASS items (overall involvement and rapport) and the CASS total score, and the SSIS, SRS-2, TASSK, and QSQ-hosted get-togethers. There was a significant correlation between change in CASS involvement and change on the SRS-2 (
r = −0.56,
p < 0.05), indicating that greater conversational involvement was associated with improved parent-reported social responsiveness. Additionally, there was a significant correlation between change in CASS involvement and change on the SSIS social skills standard score (
r = 0.74,
p < 0.01), indicating that greater conversational involvement was associated with improvement in parent-reported social skills. There was also a significant correlation between change in the CASS total score and change on the SSIS social skills standard score (
r = 0.59,
p < 0.05), indicating that greater overall conversational skills were associated with improved parent-reported social skills. Finally, there was a significant correlation between change in CASS quality of rapport and change in parent-reported hosted get-togethers on the QSQ (
r = 0.60,
p < 0.05), indicating that greater overall rapport was associated with an increase in hosted get-togethers. No other correlations were significant. See Table
5 for correlations between mean difference scores on the CASS global items and questionnaire measures from pre- to post- intervention.
Table 5
Correlations between mean difference scores on CASS global items and questionnaire measures from pre- to post- intervention (n = 13)
1. CASS Involvement | − | | | | | | |
2. CASS Rapport | 0.36 | − | | | | | |
3. CASS Total | 0.79** | 0.54 | − | | | | |
4. SRS-2 Total | −0.56* | −0.05 | −0.32 | − | | | |
5. SSIS Social Skills | 0.74** | 0.26 | 0.59* | −0.83** | − | | |
6. SSIS Problem Behaviors | −0.17 | 0.41 | 0.15 | 0.66* | −0.32 | − | |
7. TASSK-R | 0.09 | −0.30 | 0.10 | 0.43 | −0.36 | 0.11 | − |
Limitations and Future Directions
The obvious first limitation is the small sample size, which affects the number and significance of analyses. There are a few other notable limitations. First, participants in this study were not randomized to a treatment or waitlist control group. Rather, this study used a convenience sample to assess the preliminary effectiveness of the PEERS social skills intervention with primarily Latinx families, which may have led to a potential selection bias (i.e., only highly motivated families enrolled). Future research should incorporate a randomized approach to validate the PEERS intervention with Latinx and diverse populations.
Second, for the purpose of ease and efficiency, the research confederates used in this study for the CASS were college students and were therefore not same-age peers. Participants may have perceived them as “adults” rather than as peers. Future research should recruit students closer in age to the participants to better gauge how adolescents interact with same-age peers. Third, this study only includes pre- and post- data on the CASS, as in-person follow-up appointments were not possible for the second cohort due to the COVID-19 pandemic. Future studies should include follow-up data using the CASS to evaluate the maintenance of observed treatment gains after adolescents have had several months to practice the newly learned skills. Fourth, as previous studies have found significant differences on the CASS between TD and ASD participants without treatment (e.g., Ratto et al.,
2011), future PEERS studies should consider administering the CASS to a control group of TD participants at pre- and post- intervention to examine whether these differences decrease with treatment.
Finally, as the purpose of PEERS is to develop close meaningful friendships with same-age peers, the concepts taught in the program go above and beyond conversational skills. For example, PEERS teaches the skills for handling arguments with friends, responding to teasing and bullying, and planning and organizing get-togethers with friends. Thus, observing how participants interact with familiar peers at school or at their extracurricular activities may reflect a more accurate representation of treatment gains.
In conclusion, this study extends previous findings on the perceived and observed effects of the PEERS intervention, and provides preliminary evidence for the successful delivery of PEERS in both English and Spanish. Findings suggest that adolescents and their parents who participated in PEERS perceived positive changes in social functioning, which were somewhat corroborated by observed changes in conversational skills.
Publisher's Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.