Skip to main content

Welkom bij THIM Hogeschool voor Fysiotherapie & Bohn Stafleu van Loghum

THIM Hogeschool voor Fysiotherapie heeft ervoor gezorgd dat je Mijn BSL eenvoudig en snel kunt raadplegen. Je kunt je links eenvoudig registreren. Met deze gegevens kun je thuis, of waar ook ter wereld toegang krijgen tot Mijn BSL. Heb je een vraag, neem dan contact op met helpdesk@thim.nl.

Registreer

Om ook buiten de locaties van THIM, thuis bijvoorbeeld, van Mijn BSL gebruik te kunnen maken, moet je jezelf eenmalig registreren. Dit kan alleen vanaf een computer op een van de locaties van THIM.

Eenmaal geregistreerd kun je thuis of waar ook ter wereld onbeperkt toegang krijgen tot Mijn BSL.

Login

Als u al geregistreerd bent, hoeft u alleen maar in te loggen om onbeperkt toegang te krijgen tot Mijn BSL.

Top
Gepubliceerd in:

01-05-2007 | Original Article

Using geometry to specify location: implications for spatial coding in children and nonhuman animals

Auteurs: Stella F. Lourenco, Janellen Huttenlocher

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 3/2007

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

The study of spatial cognition has benefited greatly from a technique known as the disorientation procedure. This procedure was originally used with rats to show that they relied on the geometry of an enclosed space to locate a target hidden in that space. Disorientation has since been used with a variety of mobile animals, including human children, to examine the coding of geometric information. Here, we focus mostly on our recent work with young children. We examine a set of issues concerning reorientation—namely, the nature of geometric coding, the processes invoked by disorientation, and the developmental origins of using geometric information to determine location. We have employed a variety of methods to examine these issues; the methods include analyzing search behaviors, using spaces of different shapes, varying viewing position, and comparing different disorientation procedures. The implications for how children and nonhuman animals code geometric information are discussed.
Literatuur
go back to reference Bai, D. L., & Bertenthal, B. I. (1992). Locomotor status and the development of spatial search skills. Child Development, 63, 215–226.PubMedCrossRef Bai, D. L., & Bertenthal, B. I. (1992). Locomotor status and the development of spatial search skills. Child Development, 63, 215–226.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Biederman, I. (1987). Recognition-by-components: A theory of human image understanding. Psychological Review, 94, 115–147.PubMedCrossRef Biederman, I. (1987). Recognition-by-components: A theory of human image understanding. Psychological Review, 94, 115–147.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Biederman, I., & Bar, M. (1999). One-shot viewpoint invariance in matching novel objects. Vision Research, 39, 2885–2899.PubMedCrossRef Biederman, I., & Bar, M. (1999). One-shot viewpoint invariance in matching novel objects. Vision Research, 39, 2885–2899.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Cheng, K. (1986). A purely geometric module in the rat’s spatial representation. Cognition, 23, 149–178.PubMedCrossRef Cheng, K. (1986). A purely geometric module in the rat’s spatial representation. Cognition, 23, 149–178.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Cheng, K. (1988). Some psychophysics of the pigeon’s use of landmarks. Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 162, 815–826.CrossRef Cheng, K. (1988). Some psychophysics of the pigeon’s use of landmarks. Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 162, 815–826.CrossRef
go back to reference Cheng, K. (1989). The vector sum model of pigeon landmark use. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior and Processes, 15, 366–375.CrossRef Cheng, K. (1989). The vector sum model of pigeon landmark use. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior and Processes, 15, 366–375.CrossRef
go back to reference Cheng, K. (2005). Reflections on geometry and navigation. Connection Science, 17, 5–21.CrossRef Cheng, K. (2005). Reflections on geometry and navigation. Connection Science, 17, 5–21.CrossRef
go back to reference Cheng, K., & Gallistel, C. R. (1984). Testing the geometric power of an animal’s spatialrepresentation. In H.L. Roitblat, T.G. Bever, & H.S. Terrace (Eds.). Animal cognition: Proceedings of the Harry Frank Guggenheim Conference, June 2-4, 1982 (pp. 409–423). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Cheng, K., & Gallistel, C. R. (1984). Testing the geometric power of an animal’s spatialrepresentation. In H.L. Roitblat, T.G. Bever, & H.S. Terrace (Eds.). Animal cognition: Proceedings of the Harry Frank Guggenheim Conference, June 2-4, 1982 (pp. 409–423). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Cheng, K., & Gallistel, C. R. (2005). Shape parameters explain data form spatial transformations: Comment on Pearce et al. (2004) and Tommasi & Polli (2004) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 31, 254–259.PubMedCrossRef Cheng, K., & Gallistel, C. R. (2005). Shape parameters explain data form spatial transformations: Comment on Pearce et al. (2004) and Tommasi & Polli (2004) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 31, 254–259.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Cheng, K., & Newcombe, N. S. (2005). Is there a geometric module for spatial orientation?Squaring theory and evidence. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 1–23. Cheng, K., & Newcombe, N. S. (2005). Is there a geometric module for spatial orientation?Squaring theory and evidence. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 1–23.
go back to reference Cohen, L. B., & Younger, B. A. (1984). Infant Perception of Angular Relations. Infant Behavior and Development, 7, 37–47.CrossRef Cohen, L. B., & Younger, B. A. (1984). Infant Perception of Angular Relations. Infant Behavior and Development, 7, 37–47.CrossRef
go back to reference Diwadkar, V. A., & McNamara, T. P. (1997). Viewpoint dependence in scene recognition. Psychological Science, 8, 302–307.CrossRef Diwadkar, V. A., & McNamara, T. P. (1997). Viewpoint dependence in scene recognition. Psychological Science, 8, 302–307.CrossRef
go back to reference Gallistel, C. R. (1990). The organization of learning. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Gallistel, C. R. (1990). The organization of learning. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
go back to reference Gallistel, C. R., & Cramer, A. E. (1996). Computations of metric maps in mammals: Getting oriented and choosing a multi-destination route. The Journal of Experimental Biology, 199, 211–217.PubMed Gallistel, C. R., & Cramer, A. E. (1996). Computations of metric maps in mammals: Getting oriented and choosing a multi-destination route. The Journal of Experimental Biology, 199, 211–217.PubMed
go back to reference Gouteux, S., & Spelke, E. S. (2001). Children’s use of geometry and landmarks to reorient in an open space. Cognition, 81, 119–148.PubMedCrossRef Gouteux, S., & Spelke, E. S. (2001). Children’s use of geometry and landmarks to reorient in an open space. Cognition, 81, 119–148.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gouteux, S., Thinus-Blanc, C., & Vauclair, J. (2001a). Rhesus monkeys use geometric and nongeometric information during a reorientation task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 505–519.CrossRef Gouteux, S., Thinus-Blanc, C., & Vauclair, J. (2001a). Rhesus monkeys use geometric and nongeometric information during a reorientation task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 505–519.CrossRef
go back to reference Gouteux, S., Vauclair, J., & Thinus-Blanc, C. (2001b). Reorientation in a small-scaleenvironment by 3-, 4-, and 5-year-old children. Cognitive Development, 16, 853–869.CrossRef Gouteux, S., Vauclair, J., & Thinus-Blanc, C. (2001b). Reorientation in a small-scaleenvironment by 3-, 4-, and 5-year-old children. Cognitive Development, 16, 853–869.CrossRef
go back to reference Gray, E. R., Bloomfield, L. L., Ferrey, A., Spetch, M. L., & Sturdy, C. B. (2005). Spatial encoding in mountain chickadees: Features overshadow geometry. Biology Letters, 1, 314–317.PubMedCrossRef Gray, E. R., Bloomfield, L. L., Ferrey, A., Spetch, M. L., & Sturdy, C. B. (2005). Spatial encoding in mountain chickadees: Features overshadow geometry. Biology Letters, 1, 314–317.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hermer, L., & Spelke, E. (1994). A geometric process for spatial reorientation in young children. Nature, 370, 57–59.PubMedCrossRef Hermer, L., & Spelke, E. (1994). A geometric process for spatial reorientation in young children. Nature, 370, 57–59.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hermer, L., & Spelke, E. (1996). Modularity and development: A case of spatial reorientation. Cognition, 61, 195–232.PubMedCrossRef Hermer, L., & Spelke, E. (1996). Modularity and development: A case of spatial reorientation. Cognition, 61, 195–232.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hupbach, A., & Nadel, L. (2005). Reorientation in a rhombic environment: No evidence for an encapsulated geometric model. Cognitive Development, 20, 279–302.CrossRef Hupbach, A., & Nadel, L. (2005). Reorientation in a rhombic environment: No evidence for an encapsulated geometric model. Cognitive Development, 20, 279–302.CrossRef
go back to reference Huttenlocher, J., & Presson, C. C. (1973). Mental rotation and the perspective problem. Cognitive Psychology, 4, 277–299.CrossRef Huttenlocher, J., & Presson, C. C. (1973). Mental rotation and the perspective problem. Cognitive Psychology, 4, 277–299.CrossRef
go back to reference Huttenlocher, J., & Presson, C. C. (1979). The coding and transformation of spatial information. Cognitive Psychology, 11, 375–394.PubMedCrossRef Huttenlocher, J., & Presson, C. C. (1979). The coding and transformation of spatial information. Cognitive Psychology, 11, 375–394.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Huttenlocher, J., & Newcombe, N. (1984). The child’s representations of information aboutlocation. In C. Sophian (Ed.), Origins of cognitive skills (pp. 81–111). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Huttenlocher, J., & Newcombe, N. (1984). The child’s representations of information aboutlocation. In C. Sophian (Ed.), Origins of cognitive skills (pp. 81–111). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Huttenlocher, J., Newcombe, N., & Sandberg, E. (1994). The coding of spatial location in young children. Cognitive Psychology, 27, 115–147.PubMedCrossRef Huttenlocher, J., Newcombe, N., & Sandberg, E. (1994). The coding of spatial location in young children. Cognitive Psychology, 27, 115–147.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Huttenlocher, J., & Vasilyeva, M. (2003). How toddlers represent enclosed spaces. Cognitive Science, 27, 749–766.CrossRef Huttenlocher, J., & Vasilyeva, M. (2003). How toddlers represent enclosed spaces. Cognitive Science, 27, 749–766.CrossRef
go back to reference Huttenlocher, J., Lourenco, S. F., & Vasilyeva, M. (2006). Perspectives on spatial development. In L. B. Smith, M. Gasser, & K. Mix (Eds.). The spatial foundations of cognition and language. Oxford University Press, New York (in press). Huttenlocher, J., Lourenco, S. F., & Vasilyeva, M. (2006). Perspectives on spatial development. In L. B. Smith, M. Gasser, & K. Mix (Eds.). The spatial foundations of cognition and language. Oxford University Press, New York (in press).
go back to reference Jacobs, D. W. (2003). What makes viewpoint-invariant properties perceptually salient? Journal of Optical Society of America, 20, 1304–1320. Jacobs, D. W. (2003). What makes viewpoint-invariant properties perceptually salient? Journal of Optical Society of America, 20, 1304–1320.
go back to reference Kelly, D. M., & Spetch, M. L. (2001). Pigeons encode relative geometry. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 27, 417–422.PubMedCrossRef Kelly, D. M., & Spetch, M. L. (2001). Pigeons encode relative geometry. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 27, 417–422.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kelly, D. M., & Spetch, M. L. (2004a). Reorientation in a two-dimensional environment: I. Do adults encode the featural and geometric properties of a two-dimensional schematic of a room? Journal of Comparative Psychology, 118, 82–94.CrossRef Kelly, D. M., & Spetch, M. L. (2004a). Reorientation in a two-dimensional environment: I. Do adults encode the featural and geometric properties of a two-dimensional schematic of a room? Journal of Comparative Psychology, 118, 82–94.CrossRef
go back to reference Kelly, D. M., & Spetch, M. L. (2004b). Reorientation in a two-dimensional environment: II. Do pigeons (Columba livia) encode the featural and geometric properties of a two-dimensional schematic of a room? Journal of Comparative Psychology, 118, 384–395.CrossRef Kelly, D. M., & Spetch, M. L. (2004b). Reorientation in a two-dimensional environment: II. Do pigeons (Columba livia) encode the featural and geometric properties of a two-dimensional schematic of a room? Journal of Comparative Psychology, 118, 384–395.CrossRef
go back to reference Kelly, D. M., Spetch, M. L., & Heth, C. D. (1998). Pigeons’ (Columba livia) encoding of geometric and featural properties of a spatial environment. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 112, 259–269.CrossRef Kelly, D. M., Spetch, M. L., & Heth, C. D. (1998). Pigeons’ (Columba livia) encoding of geometric and featural properties of a spatial environment. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 112, 259–269.CrossRef
go back to reference Landau, B., & Spelke, E. (1988). Geometric complexity and object search in infancy. Developmental Psychology, 24, 512–521.CrossRef Landau, B., & Spelke, E. (1988). Geometric complexity and object search in infancy. Developmental Psychology, 24, 512–521.CrossRef
go back to reference Learmonth, A. E., Newcombe, N., & Huttenlocher, J. (2001). Toddlers’ use of metric information and landmarks to reorient. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 80, 225–244.PubMedCrossRef Learmonth, A. E., Newcombe, N., & Huttenlocher, J. (2001). Toddlers’ use of metric information and landmarks to reorient. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 80, 225–244.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Learmonth, A. E., Nadel, L., & Newcombe, N. S. (2002). Children’s use of landmarks: Implications for modularity theory. Psychological Science, 13, 337–341.PubMedCrossRef Learmonth, A. E., Nadel, L., & Newcombe, N. S. (2002). Children’s use of landmarks: Implications for modularity theory. Psychological Science, 13, 337–341.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Levine, S. C., Huttenlocher, J., Taylor, A., & Langrock, A. (1999). Early sex differences in spatial skill. Developmental Psychology, 35, 940–949.PubMedCrossRef Levine, S. C., Huttenlocher, J., Taylor, A., & Langrock, A. (1999). Early sex differences in spatial skill. Developmental Psychology, 35, 940–949.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lourenco, S. F., & Huttenlocher, J. (2006). How do young children determine location? Evidence from disorientation tasks. Cognition 100, 511–529. Lourenco, S. F., & Huttenlocher, J. (2006). How do young children determine location? Evidence from disorientation tasks. Cognition 100, 511–529.
go back to reference Lourenco, S. F., Huttenlocher, J., & Fabian, L. (2005a). Coding geometric information in infancy. Poster presented at the biennial meeting of the Cognitive Development Society, San Diego. Lourenco, S. F., Huttenlocher, J., & Fabian, L. (2005a). Coding geometric information in infancy. Poster presented at the biennial meeting of the Cognitive Development Society, San Diego.
go back to reference Lourenco, S. F., Huttenlocher, J., & Vasilyeva, M. (2005b). Toddlers’ representations of space: The role of viewer perspective. Psychological Science, 16, 255–259.CrossRef Lourenco, S. F., Huttenlocher, J., & Vasilyeva, M. (2005b). Toddlers’ representations of space: The role of viewer perspective. Psychological Science, 16, 255–259.CrossRef
go back to reference Margules, J., & Gallistel, C. R. (1988). Heading in the rat: Determination by environmental shape. Animal Learning & Behavior, 16, 404–410. Margules, J., & Gallistel, C. R. (1988). Heading in the rat: Determination by environmental shape. Animal Learning & Behavior, 16, 404–410.
go back to reference Newcombe, N. S. (2002a). The nativist-empiricist controversy in the context of recent research on spatial and quantitative development. Psychological Science, 13, 395–401.CrossRef Newcombe, N. S. (2002a). The nativist-empiricist controversy in the context of recent research on spatial and quantitative development. Psychological Science, 13, 395–401.CrossRef
go back to reference Newcombe, N. S. (2002b). Spatial cognition. In H. Pashler & D. Medin (Eds.), Stevens’ handbook of experimental psychology: Vol. 2. Memory and cognitive processes (3rd ed., pp. 113–163). New York: Wiley. Newcombe, N. S. (2002b). Spatial cognition. In H. Pashler & D. Medin (Eds.), Stevens’ handbook of experimental psychology: Vol. 2. Memory and cognitive processes (3rd ed., pp. 113–163). New York: Wiley.
go back to reference Newcombe, N. S. (2006). Evidence for and against a geometric module: The roles of language and action. In J. Rieser, J. Lockman & C. Nelson (Eds.), Action as an organizer of learning and development. Minnesota Symposium on Child Development Series. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, New Jersey. Newcombe, N. S. (2006). Evidence for and against a geometric module: The roles of language and action. In J. Rieser, J. Lockman & C. Nelson (Eds.), Action as an organizer of learning and development. Minnesota Symposium on Child Development Series. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, New Jersey.
go back to reference Newcombe, N. S., & Huttenlocher, J. (2000). Making space: The development of spatial representation and reasoning. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Newcombe, N. S., & Huttenlocher, J. (2000). Making space: The development of spatial representation and reasoning. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
go back to reference Newcombe, N., Huttenlocher, J., Drummey, A. B., Wiley, J. G. (1998). The development of spatial location coding: Place learning and dead reckoning in the second and third years. Cognitive Development, 13, 185–200.CrossRef Newcombe, N., Huttenlocher, J., Drummey, A. B., Wiley, J. G. (1998). The development of spatial location coding: Place learning and dead reckoning in the second and third years. Cognitive Development, 13, 185–200.CrossRef
go back to reference Pearce, J. M., Good, M. A., Jones, P. M., & McGregor, A. (2004). Transfer of spatial behavior between different environments: Implications for theories of spatial learning and for the role of the hippocampus in spatial learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 30, 135–147.PubMedCrossRef Pearce, J. M., Good, M. A., Jones, P. M., & McGregor, A. (2004). Transfer of spatial behavior between different environments: Implications for theories of spatial learning and for the role of the hippocampus in spatial learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 30, 135–147.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1967). The child’s conception of space (F. J. Langdon & J. L. Lunzer, Trans.). New York: Norton. (Original work published 1948.). Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1967). The child’s conception of space (F. J. Langdon & J. L. Lunzer, Trans.). New York: Norton. (Original work published 1948.).
go back to reference Platt, J. E., & Cohen, S. (1981). Mental rotation task performance as a function of age and training. Journal of Psychology, 108, 173–178. Platt, J. E., & Cohen, S. (1981). Mental rotation task performance as a function of age and training. Journal of Psychology, 108, 173–178.
go back to reference Presson, C. C. (1982). Strategies in spatial reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 8, 243–251.CrossRef Presson, C. C. (1982). Strategies in spatial reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 8, 243–251.CrossRef
go back to reference Rieser, J. J., Pick, H. L., Ashmead, D. H., & Garing, A. E. (1995). Calibration of human locomotion and models of perceptual-motor organization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 480–497.PubMedCrossRef Rieser, J. J., Pick, H. L., Ashmead, D. H., & Garing, A. E. (1995). Calibration of human locomotion and models of perceptual-motor organization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 480–497.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rosser, R., Ensing, S., Gilder, P., & Lane, S. (1984). An information-processing analysis of children’s accuracy in predicting the appearance of rotated stimuli. Child Development, 55, 2204–2211.PubMedCrossRef Rosser, R., Ensing, S., Gilder, P., & Lane, S. (1984). An information-processing analysis of children’s accuracy in predicting the appearance of rotated stimuli. Child Development, 55, 2204–2211.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rosser, R., Ensing, S., & Mazzeo, J. (1985). The role of stimulus salience in young children’s ability to discriminate two-dimensional rotations: Reflections on a paradigm. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 10, 95–103.CrossRef Rosser, R., Ensing, S., & Mazzeo, J. (1985). The role of stimulus salience in young children’s ability to discriminate two-dimensional rotations: Reflections on a paradigm. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 10, 95–103.CrossRef
go back to reference Schwartz, M., & Day, R. H. (1979). Visual shape perception in early infancy. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 44(7, Serial No. 182). Schwartz, M., & Day, R. H. (1979). Visual shape perception in early infancy. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 44(7, Serial No. 182).
go back to reference Simons, D. J., & Wang, R. F. (1998). Perceiving real-world viewpoint changes. Psychological Science, 9, 315–320.CrossRef Simons, D. J., & Wang, R. F. (1998). Perceiving real-world viewpoint changes. Psychological Science, 9, 315–320.CrossRef
go back to reference Slater, A., Morison, V., Town, C., & Rose, D. (1985). Movement perception and identity constancy in the new-born baby. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 3, 211–220. Slater, A., Morison, V., Town, C., & Rose, D. (1985). Movement perception and identity constancy in the new-born baby. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 3, 211–220.
go back to reference Slater, A., Mattock, A., Brown, E., & Bremner, J. G. (1991). Form perception at birth: Cohen and Younger (1984) revisited. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 51, 395–406.PubMedCrossRef Slater, A., Mattock, A., Brown, E., & Bremner, J. G. (1991). Form perception at birth: Cohen and Younger (1984) revisited. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 51, 395–406.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Sovrano, V. A., Bisazza, A., & Vallortigara, G. (2002). Modularity and spatial orientation in a simple mind: Encoding of geometric and nongeometric properties of a spatial environment by fish. Cognition, 85, B51–B59.PubMedCrossRef Sovrano, V. A., Bisazza, A., & Vallortigara, G. (2002). Modularity and spatial orientation in a simple mind: Encoding of geometric and nongeometric properties of a spatial environment by fish. Cognition, 85, B51–B59.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Tommasi, L. (2005). Geometric determinants of spatial reorientation: Reply to Cheng and Gallistel (2005). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 31, 260–261.CrossRef Tommasi, L. (2005). Geometric determinants of spatial reorientation: Reply to Cheng and Gallistel (2005). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 31, 260–261.CrossRef
go back to reference Tommasi, L., & Polli, C. (2004). Representation of two geometric features of the environment in the domestic chick (Gallus gallus). Animal Cognition, 7, 53–59.PubMedCrossRef Tommasi, L., & Polli, C. (2004). Representation of two geometric features of the environment in the domestic chick (Gallus gallus). Animal Cognition, 7, 53–59.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Vargas, J. P., Lopez, J. C., Salas, C., & Thinus-Blanc, C. (2004). Encoding of geometric and featural spatial information by goldfish (Carassius auratus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 118, 206–216.PubMedCrossRef Vargas, J. P., Lopez, J. C., Salas, C., & Thinus-Blanc, C. (2004). Encoding of geometric and featural spatial information by goldfish (Carassius auratus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 118, 206–216.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wang, R. F., & Spelke, E. (2002). Human spatial representation: Insights from animals. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 376–382.PubMedCrossRef Wang, R. F., & Spelke, E. (2002). Human spatial representation: Insights from animals. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 376–382.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wang, R. F., Hermer, L., & Spelke, E. S. (1999). Mechanisms of reorientation and object localization by children: A comparison with rats. Behavioral Neuroscience, 113, 475–485.PubMedCrossRef Wang, R. F., Hermer, L., & Spelke, E. S. (1999). Mechanisms of reorientation and object localization by children: A comparison with rats. Behavioral Neuroscience, 113, 475–485.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wraga, M., Creem, S. H., & Proffitt, D. R. (2000). Updating displays after imagined object and viewer rotations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 151–168.PubMedCrossRef Wraga, M., Creem, S. H., & Proffitt, D. R. (2000). Updating displays after imagined object and viewer rotations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 151–168.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wraga, M., Creem-Regehr, S. H., & Proffitt, D. R. (2004). Spatial updating of virtual displays during self- and display rotation. Memory and Cognition, 32, 399–415. Wraga, M., Creem-Regehr, S. H., & Proffitt, D. R. (2004). Spatial updating of virtual displays during self- and display rotation. Memory and Cognition, 32, 399–415.
Metagegevens
Titel
Using geometry to specify location: implications for spatial coding in children and nonhuman animals
Auteurs
Stella F. Lourenco
Janellen Huttenlocher
Publicatiedatum
01-05-2007
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 3/2007
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-006-0081-3