Skip to main content

Welkom bij THIM Hogeschool voor Fysiotherapie & Bohn Stafleu van Loghum

THIM Hogeschool voor Fysiotherapie heeft ervoor gezorgd dat je Mijn BSL eenvoudig en snel kunt raadplegen. Je kunt je links eenvoudig registreren. Met deze gegevens kun je thuis, of waar ook ter wereld toegang krijgen tot Mijn BSL. Heb je een vraag, neem dan contact op met helpdesk@thim.nl.

Registreer

Om ook buiten de locaties van THIM, thuis bijvoorbeeld, van Mijn BSL gebruik te kunnen maken, moet je jezelf eenmalig registreren. Dit kan alleen vanaf een computer op een van de locaties van THIM.

Eenmaal geregistreerd kun je thuis of waar ook ter wereld onbeperkt toegang krijgen tot Mijn BSL.

Login

Als u al geregistreerd bent, hoeft u alleen maar in te loggen om onbeperkt toegang te krijgen tot Mijn BSL.

Top
Gepubliceerd in:

Open Access 20-01-2025 | ORIGINAL PAPER

Czech Version of the Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness (MAIA): Psychometric Evaluation and Network Model

Auteurs: Adam Klocek, Tomáš Řiháček, Hynek Cígler

Gepubliceerd in: Mindfulness | Uitgave 2/2025

share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail
insite
ZOEKEN

Abstract

Objectives

Interoceptive awareness is crucial in mental health research, yet the psychometric characteristics of the most commonly used tool to measure it, the Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness (MAIA), have been underexplored, as the results of previous studies have been inconclusive. This study aimed to evaluate the psychometric characteristics of the Czech version of the tool (MAIACZ).

Method

Participants were sampled from seven clinical sites in the Czech Republic and the final sample consisted of 431 Czech clinical patients (75% women, Mage = 39.20 ± 11.04 years), suffering from various psychiatric diagnoses. Patients were measured weekly across 8 weeks. A combination of factor analysis and network models was employed.

Results

Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) supported the commonly used eight-factor structure of the MAIA, consistent with the original English version, though subscales Not distracting, Not worrying, and Noticing showed poor internal consistency. Invariance across gender, age, and measurement waves was confirmed using multigroup CFA. Convergence validity was established through correlations between MAIACZ subscales and measures of anxiety, depression, alexithymia, and symptom acceptance. A bootstrapped dynamic network model, conducted over 8 weekly measurement waves, showed the dynamic associations between MAIA subscales and its relation to wellbeing.

Conclusions

Contrary to the theoretical model, the temporal graph revealed that the main source nodes were related to the subjective mindset towards interoception, such as the tendency not to worry or distract from bodily signals, rather than the neutral perception of interoceptive stimuli. Well-being was also a prerequisite rather than a consequence of interoceptive awareness-related mechanisms. The Trusting subscale, which has received significant support for its predictive effects in previous literature, was found to be related to wellbeing primarily at the between-person level.
Opmerkingen

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material available at https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s12671-025-02515-w.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Interoceptive awareness (IA) is one of the key processes regarding the mind–body interactions (Fujino, 2019). It involves perceiving stimuli from within the body’s organs, such as the cardiovascular, respiratory, or gastrointestinal systems (Leiter, 2015). This process begins with sensory awareness, enabling individuals to assess their physiological conditions. Subsequently, they interpret and evaluate these neutral signals as positive or negative, influenced by their personal appraisal and the context of these bodily sensations (Gibson, 2019). In clinical settings, IA holds significance due to its association with the mind–body processes in patients (Farb et al., 2013).
To consolidate the existing literature on IA, Mehling et al. (2012) provided an operational definition that encompasses neutral, maladaptive, and adaptive aspects of interoceptive awareness. This definition describes IA as “sensory awareness that originates from the body’s physiological states, processes (including pain and emotion), and actions (including movement).” It “functions as an interactive process that includes a person’s appraisal and is shaped by attitudes, beliefs, and experience in their social and cultural context” (Mehling et al., 2012, p. 2).
Mehling et al. (2012) integrated neurological, psychiatric, and psychological perspectives into a unified conceptual model, leading to the development of a psychometric tool known as the Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness (MAIA). This instrument was meticulously created using a comprehensive mixed-methods systematic development process, which involved a systematic review evaluating 39 previous interoceptive awareness tools. MAIA’s development utilized an iterative approach to test construction, enhancing the validity of its included subscales.
The MAIA instrument consists of eight distinct subscales. Each of these subscales focus on a different facet of interoceptive awareness. After several iterations and the final adjustments, MAIA comprises 32 items, divided into the following eight distinct subscales (Bornemann et al., 2015; Mehling et al., 2012): (1) Noticing (awareness of body sensations regardless of positive, negative, or neutral connotation); (2) Not distracting (part of the attentional response to sensations of the body connected to the suppression or avoidance of bodily sensations, while the mechanism is defined as the tendency not to ignore or distract from these negatively perceived stimuli); (3) Not worrying (part of the attentional response to bodily sensations connected to a cognitive appraisal of these sensations leading to rumination, while the mechanism is defined as the tendency not to worry or not to repeatedly experience emotional distress with these stimuli); (4) Attention regulation (ability to control and change the scope of focus of attention to bodily sensations); (5) Trusting (trusting the bodily sensations and perceiving the body as safe or trustworthy); (6) Emotional awareness (derived from the mind–body integration approach; awareness that bodily signals and emotional states are related); (7) Self-regulation (derived from self-regulatory aspects of the mind–body integration approach to IA; ability to reduce distress by attention to bodily sensations); (8) Body listening (derived from self-regulatory aspects of the mind–body integration approach to IA; active listening of bodily signals for gaining insight).
The previous psychometric studies have yielded varying results regarding the number of factors in the final model. Most studies have confirmed the eight-factor structure proposed by Mehling et al. (2012), which demonstrated the best fit in American English (Brown et al., 2017; Mehling et al., 2012), German (Bornemann et al., 2015; Reis, 2019), Chilean-Spanish (Valenzuela-Moguillansky & Reyes-Reyes, 2015), Chinese (Lin et al., 2017), and Italian (Calì et al., 2015) versions of MAIA. In contrast, alternative factor structures have emerged in Japanese (Shoji et al., 2018), Korean (Gim et al., 2016), and Lithuanian (Baranauskas et al., 2016) studies, including various six-factor models. Portuguese studies proposed a seven-factor model (Machorrinho et al., 2019) and a four-factor model (Salvador et al., 2019), while Bahasa-Malay studies suggested a three-factor structure (Todd et al., 2020). Moreover, one study combined the factors of Self-regulation and Body listening into one (Machorrinho et al., 2019).
Some researchers have chosen to explore also a unidimensional model, where all items represent a single undifferentiated interoceptive awareness construct, as demonstrated by Gim et al. (2016) and Mehling et al. (2012). Some studies have continued to employ MAIA tool unidimensionally, such as Paterson et al. (2017) and Duncan et al. (2017), even though unidimensional model had previously shown inadequate fit.
The previous literature also offers alternative MAIA factor structures. One proposed perspective suggests that Emotional awareness, Self-regulation, and Body listening could be components of a second-order factor named Awareness of mind–body integration. Additionally, Not worrying and Not distracting subscales might be understood as components of another second-order factor labeled Emotional reactions and attention responses to bodily sensations (Reis, 2019). Furthermore, Hanley et al. (2017) identified two main clusters of IA subscales. The first cluster, regulatory awareness, which includes Noticing, Emotional awareness, Body listening, Attention regulation, Self-regulation, and Trusting, represents observation and reactivity as essential for comprehending bodily sensations. The second cluster, acceptance in action, comprises Not worrying and Not distracting and reflects the reduction of the negative emotional impact of bodily sensations through acceptance.
From the existing literature, it is evident that MAIA domains are often not positively correlated with each other, as shown by Baranauskas et al. (2016). This suggests that the instrument does not measure a single construct or even that the theoretical model might not be fully supported by empirical evidence. The median of correlations between factors was relatively low of 0.35, even though no negative correlations emerged in the original study (Mehling et al., 2012).
The development of interoceptive awareness is considered a significant mechanism of change in psychotherapy, particularly in the treatment of psychosomatic issues (Řiháček & Čevelíček, 2020), although empirical evidence remains inconclusive (Řiháček et al., 2022). Concerning predictive validity, Hanley et al. (2017) found that all MAIA subscales, except for Not distracting, were significantly associated with psychological wellbeing. The strongest relationships were observed with Trusting and Not worrying.
Finally, a construct and convergent validity of the MAIA instrument was assessed in the previous literature. All MAIA subscales (except for Emotional awareness) and trait mindfulness domains were positively correlated in males, while only Trusting was associated with trait mindfulness in females (Todd et al., 2020). Interestingly, regulatory dimensions of IA were associated with reduced anxiety, whereas interoceptive accuracy measured by Heartbeat tracking task alone was associated with elevated anxiety (Mehling, 2016; Gibson, 2019).
In more detail, the construct convergent validity was assessed by demonstrating a positive correlation with Body listening skills (primarily the Trusting subscale from MAIA) (Mehling et al., 2012); positive correlation with mindfulness (Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire, Fujino, 2019; Hanley et al., 2017; Shoji et al., 2018; Machorrinho et al., 2019; Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale and Mindful Attention Awareness Scale, Todd et al., 2020); and body awareness (Body Awareness Scale, Fujino, 2019; Body Awareness Questionnaire, Brytek-Matera & Kozieł, 2015; Heartbeat Perception Task, Calì et al., 2015) in the previous literature. On the one hand, Trusting and Body listening had the strongest correlations with all the validation scales. Body listening subscale had the highest construct validity of all MAIA scales when only the sample of body-aware individuals was considered. On the other hand, lowest results were always demonstrated by Not distracting and Not worrying subscales.
Additionally, the convergent validity of MAIA was supported by correlations with several related constructs in the previous literature. MAIA subscales were negatively correlated with difficulties in emotional regulation (Brown et al., 2017; Fujino, 2019; Shoji et al., 2018), primarily the Body listening and Self-regulation subscales (Mehling et al., 2012); negatively correlated to anxiety (Brown et al., 2017; Machorrinho et al., 2019; Shoji et al., 2018), primarily the Trusting, Self-regulation, and Not worrying subscales (Mehling et al., 2012); negatively with alexithymia (Brown et al., 2017), primarily the Emotional awareness subscale (Longarzo et al., 2015); negatively with pain catastrophizing, rumination, or worrying (Fujino, 2019; Shoji et al., 2018) primarily the Not worrying subscale (Mehling et al., 2012); negatively with dissociation (Mehling et al., 2012), primarily the Attention regulation and Trusting subscales; and positively with acceptance (Mehling et al., 2012), primarily the Not distracting and Not worrying subscales.
The present study pursued two primary objectives. The first objective involved assessing the psychometric properties of the Czech translation of MAIA, hereafter referred to as MAIACZ. This evaluation encompassed an examination of its reliability, construct, convergent, and predictive validity, and factor structure in a sample of clinical patients undergoing a multi-component treatment. The second objective aimed to explore concurrent and temporal relationships among the MAIACZ subscales and psychological wellbeing through longitudinal network analysis of panel data. It may be important to note that the second objective was exploratory in nature, and no specific hypotheses were formally tested.
Regarding the factor structure, there were several inconsistencies in the previous literature. Therefore, we examined fit of the most practically relevant models derived from the introduction of the present study, representing the general interoceptive awareness (unidimensional; model 1), the individual correlated facets of interoceptive awareness (eight-factor; Model 2), and two hierarchical models: first, representing the general second-order factor as in model 1 together with eight first-order factors as in Model 2 (essentially unidimensional; Model 3), and second, representing a model from Hanley et al. (2017) with Regulatory awareness (first-order factors: Noticing, Emotional awareness, Body listening, Attention regulation, Self-regulation, and Trusting) and Acceptance in action (first-order factors: Not worrying, Not distracting) second-order factors (Model 4).
Existing studies suggest that treating the MAIA subscales as separate variables may be more appropriate, as they could have distinct relationships with wellbeing. Considering this, a network analytic approach became the preferred method of analysis in the present study. Network analysis is well-suited for exploring locally independent relationships between pairs of nodes, i.e., MAIA subscales and wellbeing. Details on this approach are provided in the Data Analyses section. It also allowed us to examine temporal relationships among these nodes, providing insights into the potential causal structure of the phenomena under investigation. This approach aided in understanding how the components of interoceptive awareness influence each other and, subsequently, influence wellbeing. Moderate correlations among MAIA subscales were expected (Mehling et al., 2012).

Method

Participants and Procedure

Participants were recruited from seven clinical sites in the Czech Republic between January 2018 and December 2019. Patients were undergoing multi-component treatment, predominantly involving group therapy. The therapy’s duration ranged from 4 to 12 weeks, with a median duration of 6 weeks. Group therapy sessions were integrative and similar across all clinical sites, primarily focusing on psychodynamic orientation and lasting 90 min. The data were collected in a paper-and-pencil form before treatment and once a week during treatment (usually after each therapeutic session). For more information about the treatment and therapists, please refer to Pourová et al. (2024).
The initial number of patients participating in the study at the baseline was 444. However, 13 patients did not respond to any of the items, so they were excluded from the dataset. This resulted in a baseline sample size of 431 patients (74.5% female; 95% Czech nationality; aged between 18 and 71 years, M = 39.20; SD = 11.04). Patients’ psychiatric diagnoses according to the International Classification of Diseases 10 (World Health Organization, 2004) included primarily F4# (n = 305; where the # symbol represents any number of specific diagnoses type, e.g., F41.1, Generalized anxiety disorder), F3# (n = 81, e.g., affective disorders), F6# (n = 66, e.g., personality disorders), F5# (n = 9, e.g., behavioral syndromes with physiological problems), and F1# (n = 8, e.g., psychoactive substance abuse disorders). Comorbidity was reported in 40 patients, with combinations of F4# and F6# (n = 18), F3# and F4# (n = 12), and F3# and F6# (n = 9). For more detailed demographic characteristics of the sample, please see Table 1. Furthermore, data from patients who completed the questionnaire up to the 8th week during their therapy were included in the longitudinal analyses, as there was a rapid reduction in the sample size from the 9th week onward. These 8 weeks within therapy represent the 8 measurement waves in the present study.
Table 1
Demographic characteristics of the final sample (n = 431)
Gender
 
Household
 
Women
321 (74.5%)
In partnership
211 (49.0%)
Men
108 (25.1%)
Single
89 (20.7%)
Missing
2 (0.4%)
With parents
51 (11.8%)
Education
 
Other
78 (18.1%)
Primary
19 (4.4%)
Missing
2 (0.4%)
Secondary
226 (52.4%)
Marital status
 
Tertiary
183 (42.4%)
Single
217 (50.3%)
Missing
4 (0.8%)
Married
130 (30.2%)
Occupation
 
Divorced
78 (18.1%)
Employee
184 (42.7%)
Widowed
3 (0.7%)
Entrepreneur
30 (7.0%)
Missing
3 (0.7%)
Unemployed
66 (15.3%)
Nationality
 
Maternity leave
8 (1.8%)
Czech
412 (95.6%)
Student
27 (6.3%)
Slovak
8 (1.85%)
Retired
5 (1.2%)
Other
8 (1.85%)
Invalidity leave
39 (9.0%)
Missing
3 (0.7%)
Other
25 (5.8%)
  
Missing
47 (10.9%)
  

Measures

Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness

MAIA is a 32-item measure developed to assess interoceptive awareness (Mehling et al., 2012). Each item is rated on a 6-point Likert scale, ranging from 0 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). MAIA is organized into eight distinct subscales, each focusing on different aspects of interoceptive awareness: Noticing (Items 1, 2, 3, and 4), Not distracting (Items 5, 6, and 7), Not worrying (Items 8, 9, and 10), Attention regulation (Items 11, 12, 13, 14, 15, 16, and 17), Emotional awareness (Items 18, 19, 20, 21, and 22), Self-regulation (Items 23, 24, 25, and 26), Body listening (Items 27, 28, and 29), and Trusting (Items 30, 31, and 32).
The scale was translated from English into Czech by employing a following process. First, five independent Czech translations were prepared by native Czech speakers, including a psychology student, two psychologists, and two laypeople. Second, a subset group consisting of two psychologists and the psychology student discussed these translations and synthesized them into a single version. Third, this version was back-translated into English by a bilingual native English speaker, and the translated version was compared to the original English one. Finally, the final Czech version was field-tested with five respondents to ensure the comprehensibility of the items. For the Czech and original English wordings of all 32 items, as well as item means and standard deviations, please refer to Supplement 1 in the online supplemental materials.

Outcome Rating Scale

Outcome Rating Scale (ORS; Miller et al., 2003) is a very brief measure of psychological wellbeing suitable for repeated measurement of psychotherapy outcomes. It allows patients to rate their individual, relational, social, and global wellbeing. The assessment employs four visual analog scales, each ranging from 0 to 100. Seryjová Juhová et al. (2021) reported a unidimensional factor structure for the ORS, indicating that it measures a single underlying construct. In the current study, the McDonald’s ω-coefficient for the ORS at baseline was found to be ω = 0.85, demonstrating good internal consistency.

Patient Health Questionnaire-9

The nine items of Patient Health Questionnaire-9 (PHQ-9) are scored on a Likert scale ranging from 0 (Not at all) to 3 (Nearly every day). Higher scores represent higher prevalence of depressive symptoms. Cronbach’s α of the scale was 0.89 (Kroenke et al., 2001). Even though the Czech translation of PHQ-9 demonstrated a two-dimensional solution with somatic and affective domains (Daňsová et al., 2016), the scale could be considered unidimensional (Kocalevent et al., 2013) because both factors are strongly correlated and the overall fit of Czech version was satisfactory even for the unidimensional structure (Daňsová et al., 2016).

Generalized Anxiety Disorder-7

The seven items of Generalized Anxiety Disorder-7 (GAD-7) are scored on a Likert scale ranging from 0 (Not at all) to 3 (Nearly every day). Higher scores represent higher generalized anxiety disorder severity. The scale is believed to be unidimensional with a Cronbach’s α of 0.92 (Spitzer et al., 2006).

Chronic Pain Acceptance Questionnaire-20

The 20 items of Chronic Pain Acceptance Questionnaire-20 (CPAQ-20) are scored on a Likert scale ranging from 0 (Never true) to 6 (Always true). The scale is believed to be two-dimensional divided into activity engagement and pain willingness. All pain willingness items were negatively worded. Higher scores represent higher acceptance of pain. Cronbach’s α of both scales was shown to be satisfactory: activity engagement of 0.78 and pain willingness of 0.82 (McCracken et al., 2004; Vowles et al., 2008). In this study, a modified translation regarding acceptance to symptom, which is more general than just pain, was used (Klocek et al., 2023).

Brief Dissociation Experiences Scale

The eight items of Brief Dissociation Experiences Scale (DES-B) are scored on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 0 (Not at all) to 4 (More than once a day). Higher scores represent higher severity of dissociative symptoms. This scale DES-B is a modification for DSM-5 from the larger DES-R scale and psychometric characteristics have not been provided yet. However, DES-R Cronbach’s α was 0.96 (Dalenberg & Carlson, 2010).

Balanced Index of Psychological Mindedness

The 14 items of Balanced Index of Psychological Mindedness (BIPM) are scored on a Likert scale ranging from 0 (Not true) to 4 (Very much true). The scale is believed to be two-dimensional divided into Interest in attending to one’s psychological phenomena and Insight into these phenomena. All the insight items were negatively keyed. Cronbach’s α of Interest subscale was 0.85 and Insight subscale was 0.76. Higher scores represent higher psychological mindedness (Nyklíček & Denollet, 2009).

Whiteley Index

The 6-item version of Whiteley Index (WI) is scored on a Likert scale ranging from 1 (Not at all) to 5 (To a great extent). Higher scores represent higher level of health anxiety or hypochondriasis. Internal consistency for the 6-item version was not reported; however, in the present study, McDonald’s ω was 0.87. The scale is believed to be unidimensional (Veddegjærde et al., 2014).

Psychological Treatment Inventory–Alexithymia Scale

The five items of Psychological Treatment Inventory–Alexithymia Scale (PTIAS) are scored on Likert scale ranging from 1 (Not at all) to 5 (To a great extent). Higher scores represent higher alexithymia level. Cronbach’s α was 0.88. The scale is believed to be unidimensional (Gori et al., 2012).

Data Analyses

General Setting and Assumptions, Reliability

Statistical analyses were conducted using R, version 4.0.3 (R core team, 2020). Prior to analysis, items with negative wording (specifically, MAIACZ Items 5, 6, 7, 8, and 9) were reversed. Before proceeding with any further analyses, patterns of missing data were carefully examined using the “visdat” R package (Tierney et al., 2019). Moreover, missing data was handled using full information maximum likelihood (FIML) in the individual analyses. For transparency and accessibility, anonymized open data are available in the Open Science Framework repository (Řiháček, 2019).

Factorial Validity and Measurement Invariance

To assess factorial validity and measurement invariance, we conducted a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) using the lavaan R package (Rosseel, 2012) with a robust maximum likelihood estimator (MLR), which is still suitable for ordinal data, such as Likert scale items, and provides more degrees of freedom than specialized estimators like WLSMV or DLS (in MLR, thresholds are not parametrized). Missing data was considered not to be missing completely at random (Little’s test was significant); however, we employed the full information maximum likelihood (FIML) technique to handle also data that may be missing at random.
Covariances of latent variables were freely estimated, and model identification was achieved by fixing the variances of latent variables to 1. We evaluated model fit using several indices, following structural equation modeling fit criteria according to Hooper et al. (2008): (1) standardized root mean residual (SRMR) values optimally below 0.05 but values below 0.08 are still acceptable; (2) root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) optimally below 0.08; (3) Tucker-Lewis index (TLI), at least above 0.90, optimally above 0.95; (4) Comparative fit index (CFI akin as TLI), and (5) insignificant χ2.
Two additional considerations were made before analysis. First, significant χ2 is frequent in larger datasets and those without normal distribution (Saris et al., 2009); therefore, we considered any model with satisfactory values in other fit indices, even when χ2 is significant. Second, TLI and CFI indices were considered less informative when the baseline model’s RMSEA was very low (below or close to 0.158; Kenny, 2020). We estimated McDonald’s ω total and ω hierarchical coefficients (McDonald, 1999) for each subscale to assess internal consistency.
To test invariance of the final model, multigroup CFA was performed using the “semTools” R package (Jorgensen et al., 2020). Invariance was tested at different levels, including configural (no constraints between groups), metric (loadings), scalar (loadings and intercepts), strict (loadings, intercepts, and residual variance), and factor means (loadings, intercepts, and factor means) levels (Curtis et al., 2020). When increasing equality constraints on the estimated parameters does not excessively change the model fit, the model is considered to be invariant. Invariance was tested on the Models with constrained factor means and residual variances are parallel to each other, wherefore were both compared with the scalar model. The same fit indices (and their change values, Δ) as in the single-group CFA were employed. In samples of at least 300 participants, changes in fit indices compared to less constrained models needed to comply with specific rules to be considered invariant: ΔTLI ≤ 0.010 (all invariance levels), ΔRMSEA ≤ 0.015 (all invariance levels), or ΔSRMR ≤ 0.030 (only metric invariance level) and ≤ 0.010 (scalar and strict invariance level) (Chen, 2007).
Invariance was tested between genders and between two artificially generated age cohorts (younger and older according to median split). Additionally, because the network model assumes stationarity, we performed also a longitudinal measurement invariance across measurement waves, involving series of nested model comparisons with increasingly restrictive parameter equality constraints over measurement waves (Van de Schoot et al., 2012). One additional constraint was present in all longitudinal invariance models—residual auto-covariances were set to be equal across all waves for all items. Nested models were compared using an ANOVA function. Because of increasing missingness in progressive measurement waves, a FIML estimator was used.

Convergent Validity

In terms of convergent validity, MAIACZ subscales were associated with several related constructs using correlation between latent factors free of measurement error. It has been hypothesized that MAIACZ subscales are negatively correlated with depression (PHQ-9), anxiety (GAD-7), alexithymia (PTI-AS), hypochondriasis and worries (Whiteley Index), and dissociative experiences (DES-Brief) showing discriminant validity, whereas acceptance (CPAQ-20) and psychological mindedness (BIPM) are positively correlated with MAIACZ subscales showing convergent validity. In this study, the second subscale of CPAQ-20 was reversed; therefore, it represents pain willingness. Also, the second subscale of BIPM was reversed; therefore, it represents insight.

Longitudinal Network Modeling

Psychological network models are increasingly often utilized in psychometric literature (Epskamp et al., 2020). A network is a graphical representation of a set of variables (labeled as nodes) and interconnections between these variables (labeled as links/edges). Psychometric networks usually interconnect items of a specific scale or symptoms as nodes and estimated partial correlations between them as edges. Unlike factor analysis, network models do not require assumption of any latent variable allowing for the exploration of direct associations between nodes, exploration of various patterns in the data structures and of specific network characteristics such as node centrality (Epskamp et al., 2018a, 2018b). Given the edges are often based on partial correlations, adding or omitting a particular node from a network might yield slightly different results. Network models are relatively sensitive to small effects and rich information might be difficult to interpret. Therefore, some forms of network regularization are often considered to increase the specificity and reduce the potential noise in the data, such as pruning of non-significant edges (Epskamp et al., 2020). To increase the stability of the network structure, non-parametric bootstrapping is often employed.
In the present study, we did not have sufficient power for longitudinal network models computed on the item-level; however, previously extracted factor scores or subscale composite scores could be used as input data to the network model as well (Epskamp, 2020). Moreover, based on the theoretical model from Mehling et al. (2012), MAIACZ subscales represent several distinct facets of which IA consists of. Network models are more suitable methods to explore how these subscales influence each other and evolve over time together than structural equation modeling alternatives (Epskamp, 2020; Epskamp et al., 2018a, 2018b). In this study, a dynamic latent variable model for panel data (with lag-1) was used (psychonetrics, Epskamp et al., 2020). Using this model, we considered data across all 8 measurement waves and employed the “nlminb” optimizer. There is an assumption of a multivariate Gaussian density of variables and requirement of stationarity. By adding a wellbeing node into the network model of all eight MAIACZ subscales, a predictive validity of the instrument could be tested in an innovative way, exploring which of the subscales are associated with the wellbeing outcome. This method is suitable for demonstrating, which of the MAIACZ subscales possess a practical utility by predicting better treatment outcomes over time.
To address the missing data, we used a FIML estimator. The network model was defined as a multi-level graphical vector-autoregression model with random effects on the mean structure (Epskamp, 2020). We computed two network models: (1) one focused on interoceptive awareness alone (all MAIACZ subscales) and (2) another where we added wellbeing to the MAIACZ subscales. The first network model was reported in Supplement 5a, 5b, and 5c. It was not reported in the main text of the present study because it wielded the same associations as the second network model. We extracted detrended latent factor scores from all MAIACZ subscales (Model 2a) and the ORS for each of the 8 measurement waves. These scores served as input to the longitudinal network model, which was initially fitted as a saturated Gaussian graphical model and then pruned of all non-significant associations (α-level = 0.01). To enhance the stability of the estimated parameters, the saturated pruned model was further bootstrapped with 100 resamples with replacement to create 90% confidence intervals around the mean estimates.
The output of the longitudinal network model was divided into three separate networks, each graphically represented in separated plots. First, a temporal network incorporating both vector-autoregressive (van der Krieke et al., 2015) and cross-lagged effects (Selig & Little, 2012) represent within-patient temporal partial directed correlations from a previous measurement wave (Epskamp, 2020). This network shows which IA facets measured in 1 week during therapy temporally precede other IA facets measured the following week during therapy. Second, contemporaneous network incorporates within-patient residual partial correlations in the same measurement wave controlled for all effects from the temporal network (Epskamp et al., 2018a, 2018b). This network shows which IA facets are inter-related at the within-person level regardless of the measurement wave. Third, a between-person network incorporated partial correlations between mean structure of nodes varying across patients. All three networks were assessed for centrality indices (see Supplement 8). The node is understood as central, when (1) having strong relationships with other nodes (edge strength); (2) being often in the shortest path between other nodes (closeness); and (3) being often in between two other nodes (betweenness) (Costantini et al., 2019; Epskamp et al., 2018a, 2018b).

Results

Factor Structure

The fit of Models 1 (unidimensional), 3 (single second-order factor), and 4 (two second-order factors) was insufficient, as shown in Table 2. In the unidimensional factor solution, when examining factor loadings, items from Not distracting and Not worrying subscales had notably low and insignificant loadings. In contrast, the eight-factor structure in model 2 provided the best fit of the tested models (see also in Table 2), even though still not satisfactory. All factor loadings were significant, ranging from 0.325 (Item 10) to 0.917 (Item 31). For model parameters, see Table 3, and a corresponding figure of the model is provided in Supplement 2 (online supplemental materials). The TLI and CFI fit indices were slightly lower than 0.90. The RMSEA was in an acceptable range. However, it is worth noting that the baseline model’s RMSEA was unusually low at 0.160, which complicates the interpretation of the TLI and CFI fit indices (for explanation, please see Kenny, 2020).
Table 2
Fit indices among factor structures (MAIA, n = 431)
Model
χ2
df
TLI
CFI
SRMR
RMSEA
Unidimensional
Model 1
(unidimensional, 32 items)
2707.88**
464
0.48
0.51
0.106
0.116
[.112; .120]
Eight-factor
Model 2
(eight subscales, original MAIA)
1070.55**
436
0.85
0.87
0.077
0.063
[0.058; 0.068]
Model 2a
(Model 2 with item 13 ~ ~ item 14)
1015.16**
435
0.85
0.87
0.076
0.060
[0.055; 0.065]
Hierarchical
Model 3
(1 s-order + 8 first-order factors)
1232.82**
456
0.81
0.83
0.093
0.068
[0.064; 0.073]
Model 4
(Hanley et al. (2017): 2 s-order + 8 first-order factors)
1225.24**
455
0.81
0.83
0.092
0.068
[0.063; 0.073]
SRMR standardized root mean residual, RMSEA robust root mean square error of approximation, TLI robust Tucker-Lewis index, CFI robust Confirmatory fit index. Baseline model’s RMSEA = 0.192. ** p < 0.01
Table 3
Final model CFA parameters (Model 2a)
MAIA items
F1
F2
F3
F4
F5
F6
F7
F8
ε
Item 1
0.73
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
0.46
Item 2
0.78
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
0.39
Item 3
0.44
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
0.81
Item 4
0.49
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
0.76
Item 5
-
0.54
-
-
-
-
-
-
0.71
Item 6
-
0.72
-
-
-
-
-
-
0.49
Item 7
-
0.56
-
-
-
-
-
-
0.69
Item 8
-
-
0.73
-
-
-
-
-
0.46
Item 9
-
-
0.89
-
-
-
-
-
0.21
Item 10
-
-
0.32
-
-
-
-
-
0.89
Item 11
-
-
-
0.52
-
-
-
-
0.73
Item 12
-
-
-
0.55
-
-
-
-
0.70
Item 13
-
-
-
0.49
-
-
-
-
0.76
Item 14
-
-
-
0.76
-
-
-
-
0.43
Item 15
-
-
-
0.79
-
-
-
-
0.38
Item 16
-
-
-
0.73
-
-
-
-
0.46
Item 17
-
-
-
0.71
-
-
-
-
0.50
Item 18
-
-
-
-
0.50
-
-
-
0.75
Item 19
-
-
-
-
0.52
-
-
-
0.73
Item 20
-
-
-
-
0.76
-
-
-
0.43
Item 21
-
-
-
-
0.76
-
-
-
0.42
Item 22
-
-
-
-
0.75
-
-
-
0.44
Item 23
-
-
-
-
-
0.49
-
-
0.76
Item 24
-
-
-
-
-
0.67
-
-
0.55
Item 25
-
-
-
-
-
0.80
-
-
0.36
Item 26
-
-
-
-
-
0.84
-
-
0.29
Item 27
-
-
-
-
-
-
0.73
-
0.47
Item 28
-
-
-
-
-
-
0.79
-
0.37
Item 29
-
-
-
-
-
-
0.83
-
0.31
Item 30
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
0.84
0.29
Item 31
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
0.92
0.16
Item 32
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
0.71
0.50
ω
0.71
0.64
0.72
0.83
0.80
0.81
0.83
0.87
 
F8
0.11
0.05
0.17
0.45
0.28
0.43
0.44
  
F7
0.57
0.12
−0.21
0.66
0.56
0.53
   
F6
0.18
−0.13
0.12
0.59
0.40
    
F5
0.59
0.05
−0.24
0.47
     
F4
0.47
0.02
−0.02
      
F3
−0.44
−0.22
       
F2
0.23
        
MAIA Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness
F1 = Noticing, F2 = Not distracting, F3 = Not worrying, F4 = Attention regulation,
F5 = Emotional awareness, F6 = Self-regulation, F7 = Body listening, F8 = Trusting,
ω = McDonald’s omega; the allowed residual covariance between Items 13 and 14 was r = 0.41
According to modification indices, assigning Item 18 successively to Body listening or to Attention regulation would improve the model fit. Another suggestion was to allow residual correlation between Items 16 and 17 or between Items 13 and 14, all of which were representing the diverse attention regulation mechanisms. These modifications were considered to make the model align better with the theoretical representation of the IA construct. However, Reis (2019) criticized excessive usage of modification indices to improve the model fit of MAIA. He recommended freeing only one parameter at a time and only with theoretical support to maintain the model sparsity. The residual correlation between Items 13 (“When I am in conversation with someone, I can pay attention to my posture”) and 14 (“I can return awareness to my body if I am distracted”) had been found in a previous validation study (Valenzuela-Moguillansky and Reyes-Reyes, 2015). However, there was no prior validation or additional theoretical support for the residual correlation between Items 16 (“I can maintain awareness of my whole body even when a part of me is in pain or discomfort”) and 17 (“I am able to consciously focus on my body as a whole”). Therefore, the modified Model 2a (i.e., Model 2 with a single residual correlation between Items 13 and 14) was considered the final model for the MAIACZ instrument, with an acceptable model fit. The residual correlation between Items 13 and 14 was r = 0.41, and Model 2a significantly outperformed Model 2: χ2(1) = 47.09, p < 0.01.
The internal consistency of the subscales generally was adequate (ω > 0.80, α > 0.80), except for Not distracting (ω = 0.639, α = 0.620), Noticing (ω = 0.713, α = 0.684), and Not worrying (ω = 0.719, α = 0.658). If all 32 items were considered together, internal consistency was also adequate (total ω = 0.866, total α = 0.876). Additionally, regarding Cronbach’s α, Emotional awareness (α = 0.780) and Self-regulation (α = 0.790) did not reach the 0.80 threshold, which can introduce some error proneness. It is important to mention that Cronbach’s α was reported only for comparison purposes with prior literature because Tau-equivalent model was not confirmed, consistent with the studies by Reis (2019) and Todd et al. (2020). Reis (2019) emphasized the use of McDonald’s ω over Cronbach’s α after testing both the model of congeneric tests and Tau-equivalent model.

Measurement Invariance of the Final Model

Model 2a was tested for measurement invariance with respect to age cohorts and gender groups (see Supplement 3 in online supplemental materials). The final model was fully invariant between age groups divided by median split (n = 428), i.e., younger (n = 226, age ≤ 40 years) and older (n = 202, age > 40 years) up to the factor means and strict levels. However, when analyzing invariance among both gender groups, we encountered a negative error variance or a Heywood case. After reviewing the Likert response options, we discovered that Option 5 was rarely used by the study sample, resulting in missing responses. To address this, we have decided to merge both response Options 4 and 5 to reduce the number of estimated parameters. Following this adjustment, Model 2a demonstrated invariance between gender groups (n = 429), male (n = 108), and female (n = 321) up to the factor means and strict level.
Model 2a was further subjected to longitudinal invariance testing (see Supplement 4 in online supplemental materials). Due to the limited data size, the statistical test lacked the power to perform longitudinal measurement invariance analysis within the full model, which included all eight subscales and 8 measurement waves. Therefore, we performed the analysis for each MAIACZ subscale separately. According to Chen’s (2007) fit criteria, Attention regulation, Emotional awareness, and Self-regulation subscales were deemed longitudinally invariant up to a strict level. For the remaining MAIACZ subscales, invariance was established up to the scalar level, with a caveat regarding Trusting and Not worrying subscales, as these models did not yield positive definite results.

Convergent Validity

In terms of convergent or discriminant validity (Table 4), MAIACZ domains were overall negatively correlated with depression and anxiety. The exception was Noticing subscale which correlated positively with depression and anxiety. Furthermore, Not distracting and Emotional awareness did not correlate with depression and anxiety at all. Alexithymia was also usually negatively correlated with MAIACZ subscales, except of Noticing, Emotional awareness, and Not worrying, which were unrelated to alexithymia. Relatively strong negative correlation was found between Not worrying and hypochondriasis. This adds certain credibility to the otherwise less optimally functioning Not worrying subscale. Trusting and Self-regulation were also negatively correlated with hypochondriasis, whereas unexpectedly, the Noticing and Not distracting subscales were related to the hypochondriasis positively. Dissociative experiences correlated negatively only with Trusting and Not distracting (although significant, the correlation of r = 0.15 was expected to be higher) and positively with Noticing. Psychological mindedness subscales, interest and insight, correlated positively with all MAIACZ domains, except for Not worrying. Acceptance was associated with all MAIACZ domains positively, except of Noticing which was correlated negatively. The pain willingness domain of acceptance showed many non-significant associations. However, this could be related to the overall lower reliability of this subscale.
Table 4
Correlations between MAIA subscales and other measures to assess convergent validity between latent factors (n = 294)
 
PHQ-9
GAD-7
CPAQ, Activity engagement
CPAQ. Pain willingness
DES-B
BIPM, Interest
BIPM, Insight
WI
PTI-AS
Reliability ω
0.84
0.87
0.82
0.69
0.64
0.80
0.74
0.87
0.87
Noticing
0.18*
0.15*
 − 0.15*
 − 0.23**
0.24**
0.34***
0.27**
0.34***
 − 0.13
Not distracting
 − 0.10
 − 0.04
 − 0.01
 − 0.09
 − 0.15*
0.34***
0.39***
0.26**
 − 0.30***
Not worrying
 − 0.16*
 − 0.41***
0.23**
0.41***
 − 0.12
 − 0.01
0.06
 − 0.68***
0.09
Attention regulation
 − 0.19**
 − 0.16*
0.25**
0.16
 − 0.09
0.41***
0.35***
 − 0.04
 − 0.30***
Emotional awareness
 − 0.08
 − 0.03
0.16*
 − 0.10
0.10
0.40***
0.19*
0.04
 − 0.10
Self-regulation
 − 0.26***
 − 0.23**
0.27***
0.23*
 − 0.06
0.35***
0.28***
 − 0.17*
 − 0.18**
Body listening
 − 0.24**
 − 0.19**
0.17*
0.03
0.02
0.52***
0.43***
0.04
 − 0.24**
Trusting
 − 0.43***
 − 0.45***
0.28**
 − 0.37***
 − 0.31***
0.37***
0.38***
 − 0.32***
 − 0.23**
Correlations were computed between latent factors. *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001

Longitudinal Network Model

Latent variables of MAIACZ subscales (Model 2a) together with the latent variable of ORS (unidimensional) were included as nodes within the longitudinal network. Longitudinal associations between variables with 1-week lag were estimated using a panel data design. The initial saturated model showed adequate fit, χ2(2520) = 4880.43, p < 0.001, TLI = 0.910, CFI = 0.911, RMSEA = 0.047 95% CI [0.045–0.049], AIC = 35721.77. The model was then pruned of non-significant edges (α = 0.01) by fixing them to zero. The fit of the pruned model, χ2(2605) = 5128.94, p < 0.001, TLI = 0.907, CFI = 0.905, RMSEA = 0.047 95% CI [0.045–0.049], AIC = 35800.27, was significantly worse, compared to the only saturated model, χ2(85) = 248.51, p < 0.001. Nevertheless, we still consider the fit as excellent. Graphs of mean bootstrapped temporal, contemporaneous, and between-person networks are depicted in Fig. 1. Numeric values of edges with bootstrapped confidence intervals are reported in Supplements 6 and 7. Centrality indices of all three networks are reported in Supplement 8.
In the temporal network, only weak relationships were observed. Given the directed nature of this network, we could identify several Granger causal pathways. Not distracting and Not worrying subscales emerged as key independent predictors of other nodes in the network. Patients who were less likely to distract themselves from perceiving bodily sensations (less passive suppression of interoceptive sensations or avoidance) reported increased Self-regulation (r = 0.075), Attention regulation (r = 0.044), and Body listening (r = 0.037) in the following week. Similarly, patients who tended not to worry about bodily sensations (less ruminative cognitive appraisal) reported increases in Self-regulation (r = 0.032), Attention regulation (r = 0.029), Body listening (r = 0.022), and Trusting (r = 0.052) in the subsequent week. Trusting appeared less connected with other IA mechanisms and was considered as potential “causal” endpoint or sink node in the network. The only way to increase the level of seeing bodily sensations as helpful indicators of the condition of the body in our data was through reduction of worrying (ruminative judgmental thinking) of bodily sensations.
For successful connection to one’s body, Body listening, a patient needs the ability of Self-regulation (r = 0.055), which could serve as a potential mediator or transitional node of the influence emerging from Not distracting and Not worrying. Self-regulation and Body listening represent the mind–body integration strategies and were the most central nodes in the temporal network in terms of edge strength and betweenness, respectively. After self-regulation, an interesting feedback loop emerged between Noticing and Body listening. As could be seen from the graph, Noticing is further important for increased Emotional awareness (r = 0.076), another sink node in the temporal network.
Regarding predictive validity, none of the MAIACZ subscales as measured in the previous week predicted wellbeing in the following week, indicating limited support for the directed predictive validity of MAIACZ in this timeframe. Well-being itself showed stability over time and some independence from the MAIACZ construct. There was also a directed association between wellbeing and Not worrying (r = 0.057), suggesting that the relationship could also be vice versa and wellbeing might also predict IA.
In the contemporaneous network, a multitude of associations emerged that were not dependent on the weekly measurement timescale. The most central node was again Self-regulation (in terms of edge strength), integrating the entire network. The highly interconnected core of beneficial IA consisted of a triad of skills identified also as the core of mindfulness: Body listening, Self-regulation, and Attention regulation, which were independently positively associated with Trusting. However, several negative partial correlations were found in the contemporaneous network, particularly for the Noticing, Not distracting, and Not worrying subscales. Clearly, these three nodes behaved differently than the rest of the network. Both subscales representing emotional reaction and attentional response to bodily sensations were situated on the network’s margin rather than in the center (based on centrality indices). When compared to the temporal network, associations between Not distracting and both Self-regulation and Attention regulation changed from positive to negative in the contemporaneous network. Moreover, Noticing was most often in between two other nodes (highest betweenness centrality). Regarding predictive validity in the contemporaneous network, four MAIACZ subscales were independently associated with wellbeing: Self-regulation (r = 0.093), Trusting (r = 0.076), Emotional awareness (r = 0.083), and Not worrying (r = 0.094). However, these associations were small.
In the between-person network, many relationships were consistent with the contemporaneous network, but some differences were observed. Noticing and Body listening no longer exhibited an association, and a novel negative association emerged between Emotional awareness and Attention regulation (r = − 0.246). Attention regulation was the most central node in the between-person network and appeared relevant for wellbeing through Trusting. Specifically, patients with higher general levels of Trusting were associated with better wellbeing. In the between-person network, only Trusting was independently associated with wellbeing (r = 0.441).

Discussion

This study aimed to validate the Czech version of the Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness (MAIACZ) and investigate the relationships between MAIACZ subscales and psychological wellbeing using longitudinal network analysis. In general, our data supported the multi-dimensional nature of MAIACZ. Any usage of MAIACZ as a unidimensional scale is not recommended.
The original eight-factor structure of MAIA, as proposed by Mehling et al. (2012), was confirmed in the Czech version. Only one modification was made to the model, allowing for a residual correlation between Items 13 and 14 (Model 2a), which was in line with previous research (Valenzuela-Moguillansky and Reyes-Reyes, 2015) and the rationale for the measure’s development (Mehling et al., 2012). Even though, overall, the fit of the eight-factor model in the present study was not convincing, all items loaded satisfactorily on their theoretical constructs, the model successfully passed tests for measurement invariance across median split age groups, genders, and measurement waves, and finally, the model fit in our study was also comparable with the original standardization study by Mehling et al. (2012). Moreover, measurement invariance of the final model in this study was demonstrated up to the strict level between age cohorts and gender groups, and up to the scalar level between measurement waves (longitudinal stability) for all subscales.
Nevertheless, several problems with this model occurred. We suggest further investigation of Item 18, which worsened the model fit, and future studies using MAIACZ might want to omit the item from the Emotional awareness subscale. Furthermore, contrary to Paterson et al. (2017) and Duncan et al. (2017), our study did not support using MAIA as a unidimensional measure. All subdomains except Not distracting, Not worrying, and Noticing had sustainable internal consistency at the baseline. The low consistency of these three subscales is most likely not unique to the Czech translation. In fact, psychometric evaluations in various languages reported even lower coefficients for these subscales than in our study—the average internal consistency estimates (Cronbach’s α) across 15 previous studies weighted by sample size showed very unsatisfactory values: Not worrying (α = 0.551), Not distracting (α = 0.574), and Noticing (α = 0.725).
Both Not distracting and Not worrying subscales were the only subscales with negatively keyed items. The negative wording could cause shared variance irrespective of the latent dimensions (Reis, 2019) based on the confusion of patients when responding to the items (Lin et al., 2017). One item from the Not worrying scale was positively worded (Item 10) and the remaining 2 items were negatively worded. The loading of Item 10 was relatively low (λ = 0.32) compared to other MAIACZ items. Given the previous information, Not distracting and Not worrying could be recognized as different but related constructs to IA. In fact, a new version of MAIA exists, MAIA-2, where positively worded items were added to subscales Not distracting and Not worrying. Machorrinho et al. (2019) used this refined MAIA-2 Portuguese translation and reached satisfactory internal consistency for the Not distracting scale, but still not for Not worrying.
Regarding convergent validity, MAIACZ subscales correlated with related constructs in a hypothesized manner favoring Trusting subscale over other MAIACZ subscales. Our results align with the study by Brown et al. (2017), who also found Trusting subscale to have highest construct validity in their eating disorder sample with those who highly trusted their bodily sensations also demonstrated the lowest anxiety and alexithymia (Brown et al., 2017). Interestingly, the finding that Noticing was the only scale that correlated positively with anxiety and depression is in line with the theoretical information that Noticing (IAc) was historically understood as a somatic marker of anxiety (Mehling et al., 2012). This also provides support that IS or interoceptive awareness (IAw) might be unrelated to anxiety as suggested by the MAIA authors. Construct of psychological mindedness is supposed to be very similar to IA, measuring some of the mindfulness skills. Therefore, the positive correlations found in our study supports credibility and convergent validity of the majority of MAIACZ domains, except for Not worrying.
Regarding predictive validity and network approach, the fit of the network model was excellent even after pruning of non-significant edges. However, the reader should be informed that the analysis was still exploratory. We tried to interpret the emerging independent relations between nodes in terms of IA model theories from Mehling et al. (2012) and Garfinkel et al. (2015). According to Garfinkel et al. (2015), IA consists of three major conceptual domains: interoceptive accuracy (IAc; sensing, noticing, neutral perception, the capacity to precisely capture any emerging bodily sensations), interoceptive sensibility (IS; the subjective appraisal of bodily signals—how certain are individuals in believing their IAc), and IAw (metacognition about bodily sensations—integration or consonance between objective IAc and subjective IS). The conceptual model from Garfinkel et al. (2015) is more general and less detailed than the model from Mehling et al. (2012) and, in fact, both models could be nested. To combine both theories, IA is understood as a combination of sensing (IAc) and appraising (IS) bodily somatic and visceral signals, being aware of emotional reactions connected to those signals and regulating attention, resulting in the feeling of trust in the individual body and self-regulation as a process of integration of IAc and IS (i.e., IAw).
The temporal network represents within-person autoregressive effects and cross-lagged effects with a lag of one measurement wave. Even though all the temporal effects were significant, their magnitudes were low. Low autoregressive effects (an arrow of a node pointing at itself) could be interpreted that IA strategies might be unstable over time. The highest autoregressive effects showed nodes associated with reduced psychological distress: Not worrying and wellbeing. Interestingly, nodes with the highest stability behaved rather as predictors of the rest of the network.
Based on the IA definition by Mehling et al. (2012), IA is an interactive process dependent both on perception and appraisal, while the appraisal is usually shaped by attitudes. Our results suggest that attitude/mindset to IA serves as a prerequisite for the rest of the interoceptive awareness process on the weekly timescale, predicting further metacognitive integration but also perception/awareness of bodily signals itself.
These mindsets to bodily sensations might be crucial to shifting the perception of bodily sensations in a certain way. Temporal relationships demonstrate that attitude comes first and perception (i.e., Noticing) later. All four subscales representing the key mindfulness skills (i.e., Attention regulation, Body listening, Self-regulation, and Trusting) were influenced by the Not distracting or Not worrying mindset. This was surprising given the findings from previous studies, where—based on bivariate correlations—these MAIA subscales were often less related to the remaining subscales (e.g., Baranauskas et al., 2016).
Moreover, even though Not worrying and Not distracting influenced other MAIACZ subscales in a similar manner, they were conditionally independent in the temporal network. Patients might use both or only one of these mindsets to increase other IA skills. The intervention targeting Not distracting and Attention regulation in 1 week of measurement might positively impact the function of other IA components in the following week. This is contradictory to Gibson (2019) who posited neutral perception or examination of the body in the first place and evaluation or appraisal of these indifferent bodily signals in the second.
Not distracting and Not worrying were described as different modes of the same domain of Emotional reaction and attentional response to bodily sensations (Reis, 2019). Hanley et al. (2017) understood these two subscales as Acceptance in action, which he contrasted to the Regulatory awareness (remaining MAIA domains). Both subscales are probably originating from the construct interoceptive sensibility defined by Garfinkel et al. (2015).
Self-regulation was the most important IA skill in the temporal network. Not only does it independently influence other mindfulness-like IA processes such as Body listening, but it could also mediate the relationship from Not distracting and Not worrying mindsets. Mehling et al. (2024) found that patients suffering low back pain with meditating experience demonstrated higher scores on the Self-regulation subscale (as measured by MAIA) than patients without this experience. Reis (2019) understood self-regulation as a part of a higher dimension called Awareness of mind–body integration. This skill might be in the center of Regulatory awareness defined by Hanley et al. (2017). Self-regulation might even represent the metacognitive level from Garfinkel et al. (2015)—consolidating objective perception and subjective appraisal into self-regulatory integration. One way to enhance self-regulation might be undergoing a specialized mindfulness intervention (Leyland et al., 2019).
Altogether, all effect sizes (ES) in the contemporaneous network were higher than in the temporal network. It is obvious from the contemporaneous graph in Fig. 1 that the core mindfulness or IA skills of Self-regulation, Body listening, Attention regulation, and Trusting emerge simultaneously and build strong bonds among each other. Self-regulation and Body listening subscales were originally part of the same dimension of mind–body integration in the theoretical IA model from Mehling et al. (2012). Machorrinho et al. (2019) even merged both Self-regulation and Body listening together into a single latent factor. The preference for Attention regulation, Self-regulation, and Body listening as the most central and influential nodes is in line with Bornemann et al. (2015) who conducted an analysis of the longitudinal effect across 13 weeks among the general population with one group provided with a meditative training and one control group. Overall controlled effect sizes were not high. The largest ES in Bornemann et al. (2015) was found for the Self-regulation (d = 0.72), Attention regulation (d = 0.54), and Body listening (d = 0.40) subscales, whereas Not distracting, Not worrying, and Noticing subscales were not significant.
Interesting is the position of Noticing in the contemporaneous network, which had the highest betweenness centrality. However, our data and wording of items suggest that Noticing should not be described as an a priori beneficial IA skill. Noticing, which was negatively associated with Not worrying and Trusting in our data, probably does not represent the accuracy of perceiving bodily sensations (cf. Garfinkel et al., 2015). Rather, it could be defined in terms of the somatic marker hypothesis (Damasio, 1996) as a symptom of body anxiety (Gibson, 2019). Perhaps, the more potentially threatening bodily sensations patients perceive, the more they worry and the less they trust their bodies. Also, the temporal feedback loop between Noticing and Body listening is difficult to explain. On the one hand, sensing neutral stimuli without a cognitive appraisal (Noticing) is theoretically the assumption for any other IA skill/approach (Farb et al., 2015; Gibson, 2019). On the other hand, a better connection with one’s body could help patients notice more interoceptive stimuli or perceive them in a more precise way. We could further see from the comparison of the temporal and contemporaneous graphs that the Noticing and Attention regulation emerge simultaneously, rather than successively. Those patients who perceived bodily sensations with the Attention regulation filter were characterized by an increase in trusting, self-regulatory strategies and less worrying, as compared to patients without simultaneously elevated Attention regulation.
Interesting was the shift from a positive association between Not distracting and both Self-regulation and Attention regulation in the temporal network to a negative one in the contemporaneous network. Clearly, in the longer timeframe, a less avoiding attitude might be beneficial for increased Self- and Attention regulation. However, in the present moment, patients electing to not suppress or avoid their bodily sensations might be less able to simultaneously regulate these sensations. This could be explained by a potential over-flooding of stimuli in the present moment. Another explanation could be that the suppression or avoidance of stimuli already represents a regulation strategy, and patients might interpret the tendency to let things go as they go (Not distracting), as unhelpful to actually regulate the stimuli.
In the between-person network, Attention regulation was the most important node. This is in line with Hanley et al. (2017), who found that mindful observation and reactivity are necessary for patients’ understanding of their bodily sensations. Patients who were, in general, able to regulate their attention were also able to use other beneficial IA skills in an increased manner. Emotional awareness, Not distracting, and Attention regulation were all positively inter-related through their connection to Body listening. Interestingly, the missing association between Noticing and Body listening suggests that there are two types of listening to bodily sensations: the anxious-neutral Noticing and the mindful-positive Body listening with regulatory accompaniment. Patients might tend to generally function only using one of them.
We did not find support that any of the IA skills leads to a change in wellbeing. More likely, increased well-being is the necessary precondition to the activation of the certain IA aspect (Not worrying at the temporal level). Perhaps, patients need to feel well before they can reduce worrying and before they begin trusting or listening to their body. Psychological distress associated with the worsened well-being could diminish the ability to be aware of own body. Patients with poor well-being might not be interested in mindful interoception and could interpret bodily stimuli as negative. By separating within- and between-person effects, we were able to investigate the relationships between MAIACZ subscales and wellbeing in a more detailed manner. Our data thus only partially support the findings of Bornemann et al. (2015), Hanley et al. (2017), and Jokić and Purić (2020) who found that MAIA subscales, mainly Trusting, predicted wellbeing. And wellbeing seems to behave just the opposite by possibly opening the way to IA, especially to the metacognitively understood Body listening.
In conclusion, overall, the study validated the Czech version of the MAIA (MAIACZ) as a reliable and valid instrument for the multidimensional assessment of interoceptive awareness (IA) in clinical population. MAIA is a theoretically based and empirically tested instrument for the multidimensional assessment of interoceptive awareness (Mehling et al., 2012), which was designed to separately measure the diverse nature of interoceptive awareness to differentiate among various IA aspects that can be tracked and tailored to the specific problematic component of IA of the individual patients (Shoji et al., 2018). The convenience of assessing the multiple subscales simultaneously is in the variability of measured IA components and in the possibility to distinguish between them (Bornemann et al., 2015). However, the MAIA measure should be used with caution given the emerging problems in factor structure over several validation studies including the present study.
The construct of IA is important in the clinical environment for its association with mind–body processes among clinical patients. The connection between IA and both emotional and behavioral regulation is well documented (Farb et al., 2013). MAIA was successfully used in the context of clinical populations with PTSD (Mehling et al., 2018), eating disorders (Brown et al., 2017), and chronic pain (de Jong et al., 2016). All MAIA subscales were related to body image except for Body listening (Todd et al., 2019).
The results of our study supported the validity of the eight-dimensional model of IA defined by Mehling et al. (2012). These findings offer some therapeutic implications. (1) If patients want to increase beneficial interoceptive awareness, they might need to take the right attitude/mindset towards their IA first. The mindset could be either to stop worrying about all the signals that their body produces or stop distracting from them. Mindful integration and beneficial regulation come afterward. Therapists might want to begin therapy by changing the mindset of their patients before working on increasing the perception of interoceptive stimuli. (2) Not all IA aspects are helpful. Noticing bodily sensations might be a symptom of anxiety and may prevent patients from perceiving bodily sensations in a regulated, mindful manner. (3) The well-established association between Trusting and wellbeing might be interpreted wrongly in the research literature (Bornemann et al., 2015; Hanley et al., 2017; Jokić & Purić, 2020). Prevalent psychological distress might deteriorate patients’ ability to be aware of their own body. Interoceptive awareness might be an outcome that is desired per se, which is useful after the patients already feel well. Wellbeing most probably opens the possibility to mindful Body listening.

Limitations and Future Research

The first limitation was the study sample. Similar to earlier MAIA psychometric studies (e.g., Machorrinho et al., 2019), the Czech translation was also validated in a female-dominated sample with a model fit that is not convincing. Furthermore, the sample size was relatively small. The analysis was not powered enough to estimate the network models on the item level. Therefore, latent factor means of the subscales of the final model were used instead with the possibility of being underpowered even after using subscales as nodes. Finally, patients were collected in seven clinical sites, and differences between sites were neither investigated, nor controlled for in this study. The potential differences between clinical sites might bias the results.
The second limitation was related to testing of the factorial validity. The final model was modified by allowing a residual covariance between Items 13 and 14. On the one hand, even though the modification was justified, any modification might reduce the overall generalizability of the results and subsequently also the network models utilizing extracted factor scores from this modified factor analysis model. On the other hand, the model fit of the final model was still not satisfactory and further modifications might be required if the future authors intend to use the instrument as a compact multidimensional measure of individual IA facets. Additionally, the estimated internal consistency coefficients of several subscales (i.e., Not distracting, Not worrying, and Noticing) were rather low (< 0.80). By including unreliable nodes in the network, a certain bias of all estimates is unavoidable. However, using factor scores (instead of items) as nodes filtered out part of the measurement error. Moreover, the internal consistency differed considerably across measurement waves. Furthermore, the age groups in measurement invariance testing represented arbitrary categories without any theoretical support. Finally, the recent advancements in testing model fit suggest using dynamic fit indices (see, e.g., McNeish & Wolf, 2023) instead of one-time-fits-all cutoff criteria as we used in the present study. We assume none of the tested models would reach an acceptable fit using these criteria.
The third limitation was related to the stationarity assumption. Trusting and Not worrying subscales did not have positive definite models in the longitudinal measurement invariance testing. Additionally, within-person networks assumed stable mean structure across patients over time and real clinical data seldom conform to this assumption. The stationarity problem affects also the between-person network. On the one hand, because the responses from several time points were averaged, a bias of self-report measurement is reduced. On the other hand, because these data originated from a sample of patients undergoing a therapeutic intervention, average values across all time points might not be interpretable. Also, the lag of 1 week between measurement points might be suboptimal for the IA variables. Perhaps, a longer or shorter timeframe would produce different results. The usage of experience sampling methodology might be relevant in this regard.
The fourth limitation was related to the interpretation of network edges. Edges in temporal and contemporaneous networks may indicate both real causal relationships and unmodeled nonstationarity of the mean. For conclusions about causal relationships, experimental manipulation is still needed (Epskamp et al., 2018a, 2018b). Moreover, the negative edges occurring in the contemporaneous and between-person networks could be caused by a collider included as a node in the network known as a Berkson’s bias (De Ron et al., 2021).

Declarations

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare no competing interests.

Ethics Approval

The study has been approved by the ethics committee of Masaryk University (ef. no. EKV-2017–029-R1) and has been performed in accordance with the ethical standards of Declaration of Helsinki (1964) and its later amendments.
All participating patients gave their written active informed consent prior to data collection.

Use of Artificial Intelligence

Some sentences in this manuscript have been refined with the assistance of artificial intelligence to ensure proper use of the English language, as the authors are non-native speakers. However, the first draft of the article was written even before the onset of large language models.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Onze productaanbevelingen

BSL Psychologie Totaal

Met BSL Psychologie Totaal blijf je als professional steeds op de hoogte van de nieuwste ontwikkelingen binnen jouw vak. Met het online abonnement heb je toegang tot een groot aantal boeken, protocollen, vaktijdschriften en e-learnings op het gebied van psychologie en psychiatrie. Zo kun je op je gemak en wanneer het jou het beste uitkomt verdiepen in jouw vakgebied.

BSL Academy Accare GGZ collective

BSL GOP_opleiding GZ-psycholoog

Bijlagen

Supplementary Information

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Literatuur
go back to reference Brown, T. A., Berner, L. A., Jones, M. D., Reilly, E. E., Cusack, A., Anderson, L. K., Kaye, W. H., & Wierenga, C. E. (2017). Psychometric evaluation and norms for the Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness (MAIA) in a clinical eating disorders sample. European Eating Disorders Review, 25(5), 411–416. https://doi.org/10.1002/erv.2532CrossRefPubMed Brown, T. A., Berner, L. A., Jones, M. D., Reilly, E. E., Cusack, A., Anderson, L. K., Kaye, W. H., & Wierenga, C. E. (2017). Psychometric evaluation and norms for the Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness (MAIA) in a clinical eating disorders sample. European Eating Disorders Review, 25(5), 411–416. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1002/​erv.​2532CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Dalenberg, C., & Carlson, E. (2010). New versions of the Dissociative Experiences Scale: The DES-R (revised) and the DES-B (brief). In Annual meeting of the International Society for Traumatic Stress Studies, November, Montreal, Quebec. Dalenberg, C., & Carlson, E. (2010). New versions of the Dissociative Experiences Scale: The DES-R (revised) and the DES-B (brief). In Annual meeting of the International Society for Traumatic Stress Studies, November, Montreal, Quebec.
go back to reference Daňsová, P., Masopustová, Z., Hanáčková, V., Kicková, K., & Korábová, I. (2016). Metoda Patient Health Questionnaire-9: Česká verze. Československá Psychologie, 60(5), 468–481. Daňsová, P., Masopustová, Z., Hanáčková, V., Kicková, K., & Korábová, I. (2016). Metoda Patient Health Questionnaire-9: Česká verze. Československá Psychologie, 60(5), 468–481.
go back to reference Hooper, D., Coughlan, J., & Mullen, M. (2008). Evaluating model fit: a synthesis of the structural equation modelling literature. In 7th European Conference on research methodology for business and management studies, pp. 195–200. Hooper, D., Coughlan, J., & Mullen, M. (2008). Evaluating model fit: a synthesis of the structural equation modelling literature. In 7th European Conference on research methodology for business and management studies, pp. 195–200.
go back to reference Jokić, B., & Purić, D. (2020). Interoceptive awareness and mindfulness: Construct overlap and contribution to wellbeing. Empirical Studies in Psychology, 155, 20. Jokić, B., & Purić, D. (2020). Interoceptive awareness and mindfulness: Construct overlap and contribution to wellbeing. Empirical Studies in Psychology, 155, 20.
go back to reference Mehling, W. E., Chesney, M. A., Metzler, T. J., Goldstein, L. A., Maguen, S., Geronimo, C., Agcaoili, G., Barnes, D. E., Hlavin, J. A., & Neylan, T. C. (2018). A 12-week integrative exercise program improves self-reported mindfulness and interoceptive awareness in war veterans with posttraumatic stress symptoms. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 74(4), 554–565. https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.22549CrossRefPubMed Mehling, W. E., Chesney, M. A., Metzler, T. J., Goldstein, L. A., Maguen, S., Geronimo, C., Agcaoili, G., Barnes, D. E., Hlavin, J. A., & Neylan, T. C. (2018). A 12-week integrative exercise program improves self-reported mindfulness and interoceptive awareness in war veterans with posttraumatic stress symptoms. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 74(4), 554–565. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1002/​jclp.​22549CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Miller, S. D., Duncan, B. L., Brown, J., Sparks, J. A., & Claud, D. A. (2003). The outcome rating scale: A preliminary study of the reliability, validity, and feasibility of a brief visual analog measure. Journal of Brief Therapy, 2(2), 91–100. Miller, S. D., Duncan, B. L., Brown, J., Sparks, J. A., & Claud, D. A. (2003). The outcome rating scale: A preliminary study of the reliability, validity, and feasibility of a brief visual analog measure. Journal of Brief Therapy, 2(2), 91–100.
go back to reference Pourová, M., Řiháček, T., Boehnke, J. R., Šimek, J., Saic, M., Kabát, J., & Šilhán, P. (2024). Effectiveness of a multicomponent group-based treatment in patients with medically unexplained physical symptoms: A multisite naturalistic study. Journal of Contemporary Psychotherapy, 54, 47–57. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10879-023-09597-4CrossRef Pourová, M., Řiháček, T., Boehnke, J. R., Šimek, J., Saic, M., Kabát, J., & Šilhán, P. (2024). Effectiveness of a multicomponent group-based treatment in patients with medically unexplained physical symptoms: A multisite naturalistic study. Journal of Contemporary Psychotherapy, 54, 47–57. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s10879-023-09597-4CrossRef
go back to reference Řiháček, T. (2019). Effectiveness of psychotherapy in patients with medically unexplained physical symptoms: A multi-site naturalistic study. Retrieved October 31, 2023, from https://osf.io/dfrma Řiháček, T. (2019). Effectiveness of psychotherapy in patients with medically unexplained physical symptoms: A multi-site naturalistic study. Retrieved October 31, 2023, from https://​osf.​io/​dfrma
go back to reference Selig, J. P., & Little, T. D. (2012). Autoregressive and cross-lagged panel analysis for longitudinal data. In B. Laursen, T. D. Little, & N. A. Card (Eds.), Handbook of developmental research methods (pp. 265–278). The Guilford Press. Selig, J. P., & Little, T. D. (2012). Autoregressive and cross-lagged panel analysis for longitudinal data. In B. Laursen, T. D. Little, & N. A. Card (Eds.), Handbook of developmental research methods (pp. 265–278). The Guilford Press.
Metagegevens
Titel
Czech Version of the Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness (MAIA): Psychometric Evaluation and Network Model
Auteurs
Adam Klocek
Tomáš Řiháček
Hynek Cígler
Publicatiedatum
20-01-2025
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Mindfulness / Uitgave 2/2025
Print ISSN: 1868-8527
Elektronisch ISSN: 1868-8535
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-025-02515-w