Skip to main content

Welkom bij THIM Hogeschool voor Fysiotherapie & Bohn Stafleu van Loghum

THIM Hogeschool voor Fysiotherapie heeft ervoor gezorgd dat je Mijn BSL eenvoudig en snel kunt raadplegen. Je kunt je links eenvoudig registreren. Met deze gegevens kun je thuis, of waar ook ter wereld toegang krijgen tot Mijn BSL. Heb je een vraag, neem dan contact op met helpdesk@thim.nl.

Registreer

Om ook buiten de locaties van THIM, thuis bijvoorbeeld, van Mijn BSL gebruik te kunnen maken, moet je jezelf eenmalig registreren. Dit kan alleen vanaf een computer op een van de locaties van THIM.

Eenmaal geregistreerd kun je thuis of waar ook ter wereld onbeperkt toegang krijgen tot Mijn BSL.

Login

Als u al geregistreerd bent, hoeft u alleen maar in te loggen om onbeperkt toegang te krijgen tot Mijn BSL.

Top
Gepubliceerd in:

Open Access 02-07-2020 | Original Article

Regular participation in leisure time activities and high cardiovascular fitness improve motor sequence learning in older adults

Auteurs: K. Zwingmann, L. Hübner, W. B. Verwey, J. S. Barnhoorn, B. Godde, C. Voelcker-Rehage

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 4/2021

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Introduction

Older adults show higher interindividual performance variability during the learning of new motor sequences than younger adults. It is largely unknown what factors contribute to this variability. This study aimed to, first, characterize age differences in motor sequence learning and, second, examine influencing factors for interindividual performance differences.

Method

30 young adults (age M = 21.89, SD = 2.08, 20 female) and 29 older adults (age M = 69.55, SD = 3.03, 18 female) participated in the study. Motor sequence learning was assessed with a discrete sequence production (DSP) task, requiring key presses to a sequence of visual stimuli. Three DSP practice phases (á 8 blocks × 16 sequences, two six-element sequences) and two transfer blocks (new untrained sequences) were performed. Older participants conducted the Mini-Mental Status Examination and a visuospatial working-memory task. All participants finished a questionnaire on everyday leisure activities and a cardiovascular fitness test.

Results

Performance speed increased with practice in both groups, but young improved more than older adults (significant Group × Time effect for response time, F(1,5) = 4.353, p = 0.004, \(\eta_{p}^{2}\) = 0.071). Accuracy did not change in any age group (non-significant Group × Time effect for error rates, F(1,5) = 2.130, p = 0.091, \(\eta_{p}^{2}\) = 0.036). Older adults revealed lower transfer costs for performance speed (significant Time × Group effect, e.g., simple sequence, F(1,2) = 10.511, p = 0.002, \(\eta_{p}^{2}\) = 0.156). High participation in leisure time activities (β = − 0.58, p = 0.010, R2 = 0.45) and high cardiovascular fitness (β = − 0.49, p = 0.011, R2 = 0.45) predicted successful motor sequence learning in older adults.

Discussion

Results confirmed impaired motor learning in older adults. Younger adults seem to show a better implicit knowledge of the practiced sequences compared to older adults. Regular participation in leisure time activities and cardiovascular fitness seem to prevent age-related decline and to facilitate motor sequence performance and motor sequence learning in older adults.
Literatuur
go back to reference Aartsen, M. J., Smits, C. H. M., van Tilburg, T., Knipscheer, K. C., & Deeg, D. J. (2002). Activity in older adults: Cause or consequence of cognitive functioning? A longitudinal study on everyday activities and cognitive performance in older adults. Journal of Gerontology, 57(2), 153–162.CrossRef Aartsen, M. J., Smits, C. H. M., van Tilburg, T., Knipscheer, K. C., & Deeg, D. J. (2002). Activity in older adults: Cause or consequence of cognitive functioning? A longitudinal study on everyday activities and cognitive performance in older adults. Journal of Gerontology, 57(2), 153–162.CrossRef
go back to reference Bunce, D. J., Warr, P. B., & Cochrane, T. (1993). Blocks in choice responding as a function of age and physical fitness. Psychology of Aging, 8(1), 26–33.CrossRef Bunce, D. J., Warr, P. B., & Cochrane, T. (1993). Blocks in choice responding as a function of age and physical fitness. Psychology of Aging, 8(1), 26–33.CrossRef
go back to reference Ehsani, F., Abdollahi, I., Mohseni Bandpei, M. A., Zahiri, N., & Jaberzadeh, S. (2015). Motor learning and movement performance: Older versus younger adults. Basic Clinical Neuroscience, 6(4), 231–238.PubMedPubMedCentral Ehsani, F., Abdollahi, I., Mohseni Bandpei, M. A., Zahiri, N., & Jaberzadeh, S. (2015). Motor learning and movement performance: Older versus younger adults. Basic Clinical Neuroscience, 6(4), 231–238.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975). “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the conitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12(3), 189–198.CrossRefPubMed Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975). “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the conitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12(3), 189–198.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hendrickson, D. E. (1982). The biological basis of intelligence. Part II: Measurement. In H. J. Eyseneck (Ed.), A model for intelligence (pp. 197–228). Berlin: Springer.CrossRef Hendrickson, D. E. (1982). The biological basis of intelligence. Part II: Measurement. In H. J. Eyseneck (Ed.), A model for intelligence (pp. 197–228). Berlin: Springer.CrossRef
go back to reference Howard, D. V., & Howard, J. H. (1989). Age differences in learning serial patterns: Direct versus indirect measures. Psychology and Aging, 4(3), 357.CrossRefPubMed Howard, D. V., & Howard, J. H. (1989). Age differences in learning serial patterns: Direct versus indirect measures. Psychology and Aging, 4(3), 357.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Howard, D. V., & Howard, J. H. (1992). Adult age differences in the rate of learning serial patterns: Evidence from direct and indirect tests. Psychology and Aging, 7(2), 232.CrossRefPubMed Howard, D. V., & Howard, J. H. (1992). Adult age differences in the rate of learning serial patterns: Evidence from direct and indirect tests. Psychology and Aging, 7(2), 232.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hultsch, D. F., Hammer, M., & Small, B. J. (1993). Age differences in cognitive performance in later life: Relationships to self-reported health and activity lifestyle. Journal of Gerontology, 48(1), 1–11.CrossRef Hultsch, D. F., Hammer, M., & Small, B. J. (1993). Age differences in cognitive performance in later life: Relationships to self-reported health and activity lifestyle. Journal of Gerontology, 48(1), 1–11.CrossRef
go back to reference Hultsch, D. F., MacDonald, S. W. S., & Dixon, R. A. (2002). Variability in reaction time performance of younger and older adults. Journal of Gerontology, 57(2), 101–115.CrossRef Hultsch, D. F., MacDonald, S. W. S., & Dixon, R. A. (2002). Variability in reaction time performance of younger and older adults. Journal of Gerontology, 57(2), 101–115.CrossRef
go back to reference Jensen, A. R. (1982). Reaction time and psychometric g. In H. J. Eyseneck (Ed.), A model for intelligence (pp. 93–132). Springer: Berlin.CrossRef Jensen, A. R. (1982). Reaction time and psychometric g. In H. J. Eyseneck (Ed.), A model for intelligence (pp. 93–132). Springer: Berlin.CrossRef
go back to reference Lehmann, N., & Taubert, M. (2018). Exercise-induced improvement in motor learning. In H. Budde & M. Wegner (Eds.), The exercise effect on mental health: Neurobiological mechanisms (pp. 188–224). New York: Routledge.CrossRef Lehmann, N., & Taubert, M. (2018). Exercise-induced improvement in motor learning. In H. Budde & M. Wegner (Eds.), The exercise effect on mental health: Neurobiological mechanisms (pp. 188–224). New York: Routledge.CrossRef
go back to reference Li, S.-C., & Lindenberger, U. (1999). Cross-level unification: A computational exploration of the link between deterioration of neurotransmittersystems and dedifferentiation of cognitive abilities in old age. In L.-G. Nilsson & H. Markowitsch (Eds.), Cognitive neuroscience of memory (pp. 103–146). Toronto: Hogrefe & Huber. Li, S.-C., & Lindenberger, U. (1999). Cross-level unification: A computational exploration of the link between deterioration of neurotransmittersystems and dedifferentiation of cognitive abilities in old age. In L.-G. Nilsson & H. Markowitsch (Eds.), Cognitive neuroscience of memory (pp. 103–146). Toronto: Hogrefe & Huber.
go back to reference Luck, S. J., & Vogel, E. K. (1997). The capacity of visual working memory for features and conjunctions. Nature, 390(6657), 279.CrossRefPubMed Luck, S. J., & Vogel, E. K. (1997). The capacity of visual working memory for features and conjunctions. Nature, 390(6657), 279.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Myerson, J., Wagstaff, D., Poon, L. W., & Smith, G. A. (1990). The information-loss model: A mathematical theory of age-related cognitive slowing. Psychological Review, 97(4), 475–787.CrossRefPubMed Myerson, J., Wagstaff, D., Poon, L. W., & Smith, G. A. (1990). The information-loss model: A mathematical theory of age-related cognitive slowing. Psychological Review, 97(4), 475–787.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9(1), 97–113.CrossRefPubMed Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9(1), 97–113.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Swanson, L. R., & Lee, T. D. (1992). Effects of aging and schedules of knowledge of results on motor learning. Journal of Gerontology, 47(6), 406–411.CrossRef Swanson, L. R., & Lee, T. D. (1992). Effects of aging and schedules of knowledge of results on motor learning. Journal of Gerontology, 47(6), 406–411.CrossRef
go back to reference Tiffin, J., & Asher, E. J. (1948). The Purdue pegboard: Norms and studies of reliability and validity. Journal of Applied Psychology, 32(3), 234–247.CrossRefPubMed Tiffin, J., & Asher, E. J. (1948). The Purdue pegboard: Norms and studies of reliability and validity. Journal of Applied Psychology, 32(3), 234–247.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Verghese, J., Lipton, R. B., Katz, M. J., Hall, C. B., Derby, C. A., Kuslansky, G., et al. (2003). Leisure activities and the risk of dementia in the elderly. New England Journal of Medicine, 348(25), 2508–2516.CrossRefPubMed Verghese, J., Lipton, R. B., Katz, M. J., Hall, C. B., Derby, C. A., Kuslansky, G., et al. (2003). Leisure activities and the risk of dementia in the elderly. New England Journal of Medicine, 348(25), 2508–2516.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Verwey, W. B. (2001). Concatenating familiar movement sequences: The versatile cognitive processor. Acta Psychologica, 106(1–2), 69–95.CrossRefPubMed Verwey, W. B. (2001). Concatenating familiar movement sequences: The versatile cognitive processor. Acta Psychologica, 106(1–2), 69–95.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Wagner, G. G., Frick, J. R., & Schupp, J. (2007). The German Socio-Economic Panel Study (SOEP): Scope, evolution and enhancements. Journal of Applied Science Social Studies, 127(1), 139–169. Wagner, G. G., Frick, J. R., & Schupp, J. (2007). The German Socio-Economic Panel Study (SOEP): Scope, evolution and enhancements. Journal of Applied Science Social Studies, 127(1), 139–169.
go back to reference Welford, A. T. (1980). Relationships between reaction time and fatigue, stress, age and sex. In A. T. Welford (Ed.), Reaction times (pp. 321–354). New York: Academic Press. Welford, A. T. (1980). Relationships between reaction time and fatigue, stress, age and sex. In A. T. Welford (Ed.), Reaction times (pp. 321–354). New York: Academic Press.
go back to reference West, R., Murphy, K. J., Armilio, M. L., Craik, F. I., & Stuss, D. T. (2002). Lapses of intention and performance variability reveal age-related increases in fluctuations of executive control. Brain and Cognition, 49(3), 402–419.CrossRefPubMed West, R., Murphy, K. J., Armilio, M. L., Craik, F. I., & Stuss, D. T. (2002). Lapses of intention and performance variability reveal age-related increases in fluctuations of executive control. Brain and Cognition, 49(3), 402–419.CrossRefPubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
Regular participation in leisure time activities and high cardiovascular fitness improve motor sequence learning in older adults
Auteurs
K. Zwingmann
L. Hübner
W. B. Verwey
J. S. Barnhoorn
B. Godde
C. Voelcker-Rehage
Publicatiedatum
02-07-2020
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 4/2021
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-020-01351-y