Skip to main content

Welkom bij THIM Hogeschool voor Fysiotherapie & Bohn Stafleu van Loghum

THIM Hogeschool voor Fysiotherapie heeft ervoor gezorgd dat je Mijn BSL eenvoudig en snel kunt raadplegen. Je kunt je links eenvoudig registreren. Met deze gegevens kun je thuis, of waar ook ter wereld toegang krijgen tot Mijn BSL. Heb je een vraag, neem dan contact op met helpdesk@thim.nl.

Registreer

Om ook buiten de locaties van THIM, thuis bijvoorbeeld, van Mijn BSL gebruik te kunnen maken, moet je jezelf eenmalig registreren. Dit kan alleen vanaf een computer op een van de locaties van THIM.

Eenmaal geregistreerd kun je thuis of waar ook ter wereld onbeperkt toegang krijgen tot Mijn BSL.

Login

Als u al geregistreerd bent, hoeft u alleen maar in te loggen om onbeperkt toegang te krijgen tot Mijn BSL.

Top
Gepubliceerd in:

01-02-2025 | Research

The benefit of extrinsic motivation on effortful cognitive control is influenced by need for cognition

Auteurs: Qian Yang, Ruoke Xu, Lijie Zhang, Lei Qiao

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 1/2025

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Extrinsic motivation can foster effortful cognitive control. Moreover, the selective coupling of extrinsic motivation on low- versus high-control demands tasks would exert an additional impact. However, to what extent their influences are further modulated by the level of Need for Cognition (NFC) remains unclear. Thus, the current study sought to address this question. To this end, we conducted two behavioral experiments wherein cognitive control was triggered by the confound-minimized Stroop task and the NFC questionnaire was administered. Two different forms of extrinsic motivation were manipulated at the block level. In Experiment 1, extrinsic motivation was triggered by evaluative feedback. In Experiment 2, extrinsic motivation was triggered by reward incentives, while evaluative feedback was selectively coupled with low (congruent)- or high (incongruent)- control demands trials. The results indicated that two forms of extrinsic motivation (evaluative feedback vs. reward incentives) presented distinctive effects on effortful cognitive control; while their benefits on overall performance were further influenced by NFC. Interestingly, when incongruent rather than congruent trials were selectively coupled with reward incentives, not only conflict processing, but also overall performance for low-NFC participants only, benefited from this scenario. Taken together, the current study shows that extrinsic motivation can boost cognitive control, which gain was further reduced by high NFC.
Bijlagen
Alleen toegankelijk voor geautoriseerde gebruikers
Voetnoten
1
The cutoff of the current trial was determined by the performance of the previous trial: if the previous response was accurate and RT was smaller than the comparative cutoff, the cutoff for the current trial was updated using the formula: [RT(previous trial) + Cutoff(previous trial)]/2; if not, it stayed the same.
 
2
We also run the LMM by adding by-subjects random intercepts and slopes (full random effects structure), while the results showed singularity issues (see Supplementary Materials).
 
3
The initial constant cutoff and the comparative constant cutoff (which was used to determine whether the cutoff was updated or not) were smaller when the evaluative feedback was given after congruent trials (FB-C) compared to after incongruent trials (FB-I), considering that participants would have different performance levels between congruent and incongruent trials. The results showed that the mean proportion of negative feedback was higher for incongruent (64.1%) than congruent trials (54.6%). However, we do not think this is an issue as this difference between congruent and incongruent trials was comparable between FB-C (congruent: 54.8% vs. incongruent: 65.1%) and FB-I (congruent: 54.3% vs. incongruent: 63.1%) conditions. In other words, the different result patterns that may be found between the FB-C and FB-I conditions cannot be attributed to the difference of proportion of negative feedback.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Aarts, K., & Pourtois, G. (2010). Anxiety not only increases, but also alters early error-monitoring functions. Cognitive Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 10, 479–492. Aarts, K., & Pourtois, G. (2010). Anxiety not only increases, but also alters early error-monitoring functions. Cognitive Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 10, 479–492.
go back to reference Boehler, C. N., Hopf, J. M., Stoppel, C. M., & Krebs, R. M. (2012). Motivating inhibition–reward prospect speeds up response cancellation. Cognition, 125(3), 498–503.PubMed Boehler, C. N., Hopf, J. M., Stoppel, C. M., & Krebs, R. M. (2012). Motivating inhibition–reward prospect speeds up response cancellation. Cognition, 125(3), 498–503.PubMed
go back to reference Boksem, M. A., & Tops, M. (2008). Mental fatigue: Costs and benefits. Brain Research Reviews, 59(1), 125–139.PubMed Boksem, M. A., & Tops, M. (2008). Mental fatigue: Costs and benefits. Brain Research Reviews, 59(1), 125–139.PubMed
go back to reference Botvinick, M., & Braver, T. (2015). Motivation and cognitive control: From behavior to neural mechanism. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 83–113.PubMed Botvinick, M., & Braver, T. (2015). Motivation and cognitive control: From behavior to neural mechanism. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 83–113.PubMed
go back to reference Bouckenooghe, D., Vanderheyden, K., Mestdagh, S., & Van Laethem, S. (2007). Cognitive motivation correlates of coping style in decisional conflict. The Journal of Psychology, 141(6), 605–626.PubMed Bouckenooghe, D., Vanderheyden, K., Mestdagh, S., & Van Laethem, S. (2007). Cognitive motivation correlates of coping style in decisional conflict. The Journal of Psychology, 141(6), 605–626.PubMed
go back to reference Bouzidi, Y. S., & Gendolla, G. H. E. (2022). Is cognitive conflict really effortful? Conflict priming and shielding effects on cardiac response. Psychophysiology. e14169. Bouzidi, Y. S., & Gendolla, G. H. E. (2022). Is cognitive conflict really effortful? Conflict priming and shielding effects on cardiac response. Psychophysiology. e14169.
go back to reference Bouzidi, Y. S., & Gendolla, G. H. (2023). Action-orientation shields against primed cognitive conflict effects on effort‐related cardiac response. Psychophysiology, 60(12), e14407. Bouzidi, Y. S., & Gendolla, G. H. (2023). Action-orientation shields against primed cognitive conflict effects on effort‐related cardiac response. Psychophysiology, 60(12), e14407.
go back to reference Braem, S., Bugg, J. M., Schmidt, J. R., Crump, M. J., Weissman, D. H., Notebaert, W., & Egner, T. (2019). Measuring adaptive control in conflict tasks. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 23(9), 769–783.PubMedPubMedCentral Braem, S., Bugg, J. M., Schmidt, J. R., Crump, M. J., Weissman, D. H., Notebaert, W., & Egner, T. (2019). Measuring adaptive control in conflict tasks. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 23(9), 769–783.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Burgers, C., Eden, A., van Engelenburg, M. D., & Buningh, S. (2015). How feedback boosts motivation and play in a brain-training game. Computers in Human Behavior, 48, 94–103. Burgers, C., Eden, A., van Engelenburg, M. D., & Buningh, S. (2015). How feedback boosts motivation and play in a brain-training game. Computers in Human Behavior, 48, 94–103.
go back to reference Cacioppo, J. T., Petty, R. E., & Feng Kao, C. (1984). The efficient assessment of need for cognition. Journal of Personality Assessment, 48(3), 306–307.PubMed Cacioppo, J. T., Petty, R. E., & Feng Kao, C. (1984). The efficient assessment of need for cognition. Journal of Personality Assessment, 48(3), 306–307.PubMed
go back to reference Cacioppo, J. T., Petty, R. E., Feinstein, J. A., & Jarvis, W. B. G. (1996). Dispositional differences in cognitive motivation: The life and times of individuals varying in need for cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 119(2), 197. Cacioppo, J. T., Petty, R. E., Feinstein, J. A., & Jarvis, W. B. G. (1996). Dispositional differences in cognitive motivation: The life and times of individuals varying in need for cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 119(2), 197.
go back to reference Chaillou, A. C., Giersch, A., Hoonakker, M., Capa, R. L., & Bonnefond, A. (2017). Differentiating Motivational from Affective influence of performance-contingent reward on Cognitive Control: The Wanting Component enhances both proactive and reactive control. Biological Psychology, 125, 146–153.PubMed Chaillou, A. C., Giersch, A., Hoonakker, M., Capa, R. L., & Bonnefond, A. (2017). Differentiating Motivational from Affective influence of performance-contingent reward on Cognitive Control: The Wanting Component enhances both proactive and reactive control. Biological Psychology, 125, 146–153.PubMed
go back to reference Chelazzi, L., Perlato, A., Santandrea, E., & Libera, D., C (2013). Rewards teach visual selective attention. Vision Research, 85, 58–72.PubMed Chelazzi, L., Perlato, A., Santandrea, E., & Libera, D., C (2013). Rewards teach visual selective attention. Vision Research, 85, 58–72.PubMed
go back to reference Chen, J., Tan, L., Liu, L., & Wang, L. (2021). Reinforcement learning of irrelevant stimulus-response associations modulates cognitive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 47(10). Chen, J., Tan, L., Liu, L., & Wang, L. (2021). Reinforcement learning of irrelevant stimulus-response associations modulates cognitive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 47(10).
go back to reference Chiew, K. S., & Braver, T. S. (2013). Temporal dynamics of motivation-cognitive control interactions revealed by high-resolution pupillometry. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 39030. Chiew, K. S., & Braver, T. S. (2013). Temporal dynamics of motivation-cognitive control interactions revealed by high-resolution pupillometry. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 39030.
go back to reference Chiew, K. S., & Braver, T. S. (2016). Reward favors the prepared: Incentive and task-informative cues interact to enhance attentional control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42(1), 52.PubMed Chiew, K. S., & Braver, T. S. (2016). Reward favors the prepared: Incentive and task-informative cues interact to enhance attentional control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42(1), 52.PubMed
go back to reference Dignath, D., Eder, A. B., Steinhauser, M., & Kiesel, A. (2020). Conflict monitoring and the affective-signaling hypothesis—An integrative review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 27, 193–216. Dignath, D., Eder, A. B., Steinhauser, M., & Kiesel, A. (2020). Conflict monitoring and the affective-signaling hypothesis—An integrative review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 27, 193–216.
go back to reference Dixon, M. L., & Christoff, K. (2012). The decision to engage cognitive control is driven by expected reward-value: Neural and behavioral evidence. PloS One, 7(12), e51637. Dixon, M. L., & Christoff, K. (2012). The decision to engage cognitive control is driven by expected reward-value: Neural and behavioral evidence. PloS One, 7(12), e51637.
go back to reference Dweck, C. S., Mangels, J. A., & Good, C. (2004). Motivational effects on attention, cognition, and performance. Motivation, emotion, and Cognition (pp. 55–70). Routledge. Dweck, C. S., Mangels, J. A., & Good, C. (2004). Motivational effects on attention, cognition, and performance. Motivation, emotion, and Cognition (pp. 55–70). Routledge.
go back to reference Egner, T. (2008). Multiple conflict-driven control mechanisms in the human brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(10), 374–380.PubMed Egner, T. (2008). Multiple conflict-driven control mechanisms in the human brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(10), 374–380.PubMed
go back to reference Engelen, U., De Peuter, S., Victoir, A., Van Diest, I., & Van den Bergh, O. (2006). Verdere validering van de positive and negative affect schedule (PANAS) en vergelijking van twee Nederlandstalige versies. Gedrag en Gezondheid, 34(2), 61–70. Engelen, U., De Peuter, S., Victoir, A., Van Diest, I., & Van den Bergh, O. (2006). Verdere validering van de positive and negative affect schedule (PANAS) en vergelijking van twee Nederlandstalige versies. Gedrag en Gezondheid, 34(2), 61–70.
go back to reference Espedido, A., & Searle, B. J. (2018). Goal difficulty and creative performance: The mediating role of stress appraisal. Human Performance, 31(3), 179–196. Espedido, A., & Searle, B. J. (2018). Goal difficulty and creative performance: The mediating role of stress appraisal. Human Performance, 31(3), 179–196.
go back to reference Ferdinand, N. K., & Czernochowski, D. (2018). Motivational influences on performance monitoring and cognitive control across the adult lifespan. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1018.PubMedPubMedCentral Ferdinand, N. K., & Czernochowski, D. (2018). Motivational influences on performance monitoring and cognitive control across the adult lifespan. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1018.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Fröber, K., & Dreisbach, G. (2016). How performance (non-) contingent reward modulates cognitive control. Acta Psychologica, 168, 65–77.PubMed Fröber, K., & Dreisbach, G. (2016). How performance (non-) contingent reward modulates cognitive control. Acta Psychologica, 168, 65–77.PubMed
go back to reference Funes, M. J., Lupiáñez, J., & Humphreys, G. (2010). Sustained vs. transient cognitive control: Evidence of a behavioral dissociation. Cognition, 114(3), 338–347.PubMed Funes, M. J., Lupiáñez, J., & Humphreys, G. (2010). Sustained vs. transient cognitive control: Evidence of a behavioral dissociation. Cognition, 114(3), 338–347.PubMed
go back to reference Gärtner, A., Grass, J., Wolff, M., Goschke, T., Strobel, A., & Strobel, A. (2021). No relation of need for cognition to basic executive functions. Journal of Personality, 89(6), 1113–1125.PubMed Gärtner, A., Grass, J., Wolff, M., Goschke, T., Strobel, A., & Strobel, A. (2021). No relation of need for cognition to basic executive functions. Journal of Personality, 89(6), 1113–1125.PubMed
go back to reference Goschke, T., & Bolte, A. (2014). Emotional modulation of control dilemmas: The role of positive affect, reward, and dopamine in cognitive stability and flexibility. Neuropsychologia, 62, 403–423.PubMed Goschke, T., & Bolte, A. (2014). Emotional modulation of control dilemmas: The role of positive affect, reward, and dopamine in cognitive stability and flexibility. Neuropsychologia, 62, 403–423.PubMed
go back to reference Grass, J., Krieger, F., Paulus, P., Greiff, S., Strobel, A., & Strobel, A. (2019). Thinking in action: Need for cognition predicts self-control together with action orientation. PloS One, 14(8), e0220282. Grass, J., Krieger, F., Paulus, P., Greiff, S., Strobel, A., & Strobel, A. (2019). Thinking in action: Need for cognition predicts self-control together with action orientation. PloS One, 14(8), e0220282.
go back to reference Hefer, C., & Dreisbach, G. (2016). The motivational modulation of proactive control in a modified version of the AX-continuous performance task: Evidence from cue-based and prime-based preparation. Motivation Science, 2(2), 116. Hefer, C., & Dreisbach, G. (2016). The motivational modulation of proactive control in a modified version of the AX-continuous performance task: Evidence from cue-based and prime-based preparation. Motivation Science, 2(2), 116.
go back to reference Ilies, R., & Judge, T. A. (2005). Goal regulation across time: The effects of feedback and affect. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90(3), 453.PubMed Ilies, R., & Judge, T. A. (2005). Goal regulation across time: The effects of feedback and affect. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90(3), 453.PubMed
go back to reference Inzlicht, M., & Schmeichel, B. J. (2012). What is ego depletion? Toward a mechanistic revision of the resource model of self-control. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7(5), 450–463.PubMed Inzlicht, M., & Schmeichel, B. J. (2012). What is ego depletion? Toward a mechanistic revision of the resource model of self-control. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7(5), 450–463.PubMed
go back to reference Inzlicht, M., Shenhav, A., & Olivola, C. Y. (2018). The effort paradox: Effort is both costly and valued. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(4), 337–349.PubMedPubMedCentral Inzlicht, M., Shenhav, A., & Olivola, C. Y. (2018). The effort paradox: Effort is both costly and valued. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(4), 337–349.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Kalanthroff, E., Davelaar, E. J., Henik, A., Goldfarb, L., & Usher, M. (2018). Task conflict and proactive control: A computational theory of the Stroop task. Psychological Review, 125(1), 59.PubMed Kalanthroff, E., Davelaar, E. J., Henik, A., Goldfarb, L., & Usher, M. (2018). Task conflict and proactive control: A computational theory of the Stroop task. Psychological Review, 125(1), 59.PubMed
go back to reference Kool, W., & Botvinick, M. (2013). The intrinsic cost of cognitive control. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(6), 697–698.PubMed Kool, W., & Botvinick, M. (2013). The intrinsic cost of cognitive control. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(6), 697–698.PubMed
go back to reference Kool, W., & Botvinick, M. (2014). A labor/leisure tradeoff in cognitive control. Kool, W., & Botvinick, M. (2014). A labor/leisure tradeoff in cognitive control.
go back to reference Kool, W., & Botvinick, M. (2018). Mental labour. Nature Human Behaviour, 2(12), 899–908.PubMed Kool, W., & Botvinick, M. (2018). Mental labour. Nature Human Behaviour, 2(12), 899–908.PubMed
go back to reference Kuang, Y., Shi, J., Cai, Y., & Wang, L. (2005). The revision of need for cognition for the college students. Chinese Mental Health Journal, 19(1), 57–60. Kuang, Y., Shi, J., Cai, Y., & Wang, L. (2005). The revision of need for cognition for the college students. Chinese Mental Health Journal, 19(1), 57–60.
go back to reference Kurzban, R., Duckworth, A., Kable, J. W., & Myers, J. (2013). An opportunity cost model of subjective effort and task performance. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(6), 661–679.PubMed Kurzban, R., Duckworth, A., Kable, J. W., & Myers, J. (2013). An opportunity cost model of subjective effort and task performance. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(6), 661–679.PubMed
go back to reference Liesefeld, H. R., & Janczyk, M. (2019). Combining speed and accuracy to control for speed-accuracy trade-offs (?). Behavior Research Methods, 51, 40–60.PubMed Liesefeld, H. R., & Janczyk, M. (2019). Combining speed and accuracy to control for speed-accuracy trade-offs (?). Behavior Research Methods, 51, 40–60.PubMed
go back to reference Mussel, P., Ulrich, N., Allen, J. J., Osinsky, R., & Hewig, J. (2016). Patterns of theta oscillation reflect the neural basis of individual differences in epistemic motivation. Scientific Reports, 6(1), 29245.PubMedPubMedCentral Mussel, P., Ulrich, N., Allen, J. J., Osinsky, R., & Hewig, J. (2016). Patterns of theta oscillation reflect the neural basis of individual differences in epistemic motivation. Scientific Reports, 6(1), 29245.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Notebaert, W., & Braem, S. (2015). Parsing the effects of reward on cognitive control. Motivation and Cognitive Control (pp. 105–122). Routledge. Notebaert, W., & Braem, S. (2015). Parsing the effects of reward on cognitive control. Motivation and Cognitive Control (pp. 105–122). Routledge.
go back to reference Otto, A. R., & Vassena, E. (2021). It’s all relative: Reward-induced cognitive control modulation depends on context. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 150(2), 306.PubMed Otto, A. R., & Vassena, E. (2021). It’s all relative: Reward-induced cognitive control modulation depends on context. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 150(2), 306.PubMed
go back to reference Pessoa, L., & Engelmann, J. B. (2010). Embedding reward signals into perception and cognition. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 4, 17.PubMedPubMedCentral Pessoa, L., & Engelmann, J. B. (2010). Embedding reward signals into perception and cognition. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 4, 17.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Petty, R. E., Briñol, P., Loersch, C., McCaslin, M. J., Leary, M. R., & Hoyle, R. H. (2009). The need for cognition. Handbook of Individual Differences in Social Behavior, 318, 329. Petty, R. E., Briñol, P., Loersch, C., McCaslin, M. J., Leary, M. R., & Hoyle, R. H. (2009). The need for cognition. Handbook of Individual Differences in Social Behavior, 318, 329.
go back to reference Prével, A., Krebs, R. M., Kukkonen, N., & Braem, S. (2021). Selective reinforcement of conflict processing in the Stroop task. PloS One, 16(7), e0255430. Prével, A., Krebs, R. M., Kukkonen, N., & Braem, S. (2021). Selective reinforcement of conflict processing in the Stroop task. PloS One, 16(7), e0255430.
go back to reference Sandra, D. A., & Otto, A. R. (2018). Cognitive capacity limitations and need for Cognition differentially predict reward-induced cognitive effort expenditure. Cognition, 172, 101–106.PubMed Sandra, D. A., & Otto, A. R. (2018). Cognitive capacity limitations and need for Cognition differentially predict reward-induced cognitive effort expenditure. Cognition, 172, 101–106.PubMed
go back to reference Sankaran, S., Szumowska, E., & Kossowska, M. (2017). When the going gets tough, the tough get going: Motivation towards closure and effort investment in the performance of cognitive tasks. Motivation and Emotion, 41, 308–321.PubMedPubMedCentral Sankaran, S., Szumowska, E., & Kossowska, M. (2017). When the going gets tough, the tough get going: Motivation towards closure and effort investment in the performance of cognitive tasks. Motivation and Emotion, 41, 308–321.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Shenhav, A., Botvinick, M. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2013). The expected value of control: An integrative theory of anterior cingulate cortex function. Neuron, 79(2), 217–240.PubMedPubMedCentral Shenhav, A., Botvinick, M. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2013). The expected value of control: An integrative theory of anterior cingulate cortex function. Neuron, 79(2), 217–240.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Shepherd, J. (2023). Conscious cognitive effort in cognitive control. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 14(2), e1629.PubMed Shepherd, J. (2023). Conscious cognitive effort in cognitive control. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 14(2), e1629.PubMed
go back to reference Soutschek, A., Strobach, T., & Schubert, T. (2014). Motivational and cognitive determinants of control during conflict processing. Cognition and Emotion, 28(6), 1076–1089.PubMed Soutschek, A., Strobach, T., & Schubert, T. (2014). Motivational and cognitive determinants of control during conflict processing. Cognition and Emotion, 28(6), 1076–1089.PubMed
go back to reference Soutschek, A., Stelzel, C., Paschke, L., Walter, H., & Schubert, T. (2015). Dissociable effects of motivation and expectancy on conflict processing: An fMRI study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 27(2), 409–423.PubMed Soutschek, A., Stelzel, C., Paschke, L., Walter, H., & Schubert, T. (2015). Dissociable effects of motivation and expectancy on conflict processing: An fMRI study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 27(2), 409–423.PubMed
go back to reference Ullsperger, M., Danielmeier, C., & Jocham, G. (2014). Neurophysiology of performance monitoring and adaptive behavior. Physiological Reviews, 94(1), 35–79.PubMed Ullsperger, M., Danielmeier, C., & Jocham, G. (2014). Neurophysiology of performance monitoring and adaptive behavior. Physiological Reviews, 94(1), 35–79.PubMed
go back to reference van den Berg, B., Krebs, R. M., Lorist, M. M., & Woldorff, M. G. (2014). Utilization of reward-prospect enhances preparatory attention and reduces stimulus conflict. Cognitive Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 14, 561–577. van den Berg, B., Krebs, R. M., Lorist, M. M., & Woldorff, M. G. (2014). Utilization of reward-prospect enhances preparatory attention and reduces stimulus conflict. Cognitive Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 14, 561–577.
go back to reference Vermeylen, L., Braem, S., & Notebaert, W. (2019). The affective twitches of task switches: Task switch cues are evaluated as negative. Cognition, 183, 124–130.PubMed Vermeylen, L., Braem, S., & Notebaert, W. (2019). The affective twitches of task switches: Task switch cues are evaluated as negative. Cognition, 183, 124–130.PubMed
go back to reference Vocat, R., Pourtois, G., & Vuilleumier, P. (2008). Unavoidable errors: A spatio-temporal analysis of time-course and neural sources of evoked potentials associated with error processing in a speeded task. Neuropsychologia, 46(10), 2545–2555.PubMed Vocat, R., Pourtois, G., & Vuilleumier, P. (2008). Unavoidable errors: A spatio-temporal analysis of time-course and neural sources of evoked potentials associated with error processing in a speeded task. Neuropsychologia, 46(10), 2545–2555.PubMed
go back to reference Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6), 1063.PubMed Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6), 1063.PubMed
go back to reference Westbrook, A., & Braver, T. S. (2015). Cognitive effort: A neuroeconomic approach. Cognitive Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 15, 395–415. Westbrook, A., & Braver, T. S. (2015). Cognitive effort: A neuroeconomic approach. Cognitive Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 15, 395–415.
go back to reference Westbrook, A., Kester, D., & Braver, T. S. (2013). What is the subjective cost of cognitive effort? Load, trait, and aging effects revealed by economic preference. PloS One, 8(7), e68210. Westbrook, A., Kester, D., & Braver, T. S. (2013). What is the subjective cost of cognitive effort? Load, trait, and aging effects revealed by economic preference. PloS One, 8(7), e68210.
go back to reference Wickham, H., Chang, W., & Wickham, M. H. (2016). Package ‘ggplot2’. Create elegant data visualisations using the grammar of graphics. Version, 2, 1–189. Wickham, H., Chang, W., & Wickham, M. H. (2016). Package ‘ggplot2’. Create elegant data visualisations using the grammar of graphics. Version, 2, 1–189.
go back to reference Wickham, H., Averick, M., Bryan, J., Chang, W., McGowan, L. D. A., François, R., et al. (2019). Welcome to the Tidyverse. Journal of Open Source Software, 4(43), 1686. Wickham, H., Averick, M., Bryan, J., Chang, W., McGowan, L. D. A., François, R., et al. (2019). Welcome to the Tidyverse. Journal of Open Source Software, 4(43), 1686.
go back to reference Yamaguchi, M., & Nishimura, A. (2019). Modulating proactive cognitive control by reward: Differential anticipatory effects of performance-contingent and non-contingent rewards. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 83(2), 258–274.PubMed Yamaguchi, M., & Nishimura, A. (2019). Modulating proactive cognitive control by reward: Differential anticipatory effects of performance-contingent and non-contingent rewards. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 83(2), 258–274.PubMed
go back to reference Yang, Q., Paul, K., & Pourtois, G. (2019). Defensive motivation increases conflict adaptation through local changes in cognitive control: Evidence from ERPs and mid-frontal theta. Biological Psychology, 148, 107738.PubMed Yang, Q., Paul, K., & Pourtois, G. (2019). Defensive motivation increases conflict adaptation through local changes in cognitive control: Evidence from ERPs and mid-frontal theta. Biological Psychology, 148, 107738.PubMed
go back to reference Yang, Q., Xing, J., Braem, S., & Pourtois, G. (2022). The selective use of punishments on congruent versus incongruent trials in the Stroop task. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 193, 107654.PubMed Yang, Q., Xing, J., Braem, S., & Pourtois, G. (2022). The selective use of punishments on congruent versus incongruent trials in the Stroop task. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 193, 107654.PubMed
go back to reference Zhang, Y., Leng, X., & Shenhav, A. (2024). Make or break: The influence of expected challenges and rewards on the motivation and experience associated with cognitive effort exertion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 36(12), 2863–2885. Zhang, Y., Leng, X., & Shenhav, A. (2024). Make or break: The influence of expected challenges and rewards on the motivation and experience associated with cognitive effort exertion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 36(12), 2863–2885.
Metagegevens
Titel
The benefit of extrinsic motivation on effortful cognitive control is influenced by need for cognition
Auteurs
Qian Yang
Ruoke Xu
Lijie Zhang
Lei Qiao
Publicatiedatum
01-02-2025
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 1/2025
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-024-02074-0