To date, defenders of ethnically victimized adolescents have been studied as a uniform group (Abbott & Cameron,
2014). However, distinct subgroups of defenders have been found for general peer victimization (Reijntjes et al.,
2016; Yun,
2020). Thus, the present study aimed to extend the existing literature by examining whether there are also different defender subgroups in ethnic victimization incidents and whether these defender subgroups show variations in their socio-cognitive skills, views about immigrants, how they perceive their class context and social acceptance in peer relationships.
Identification of Four Defender Subgroups
The first main discovery of the present study was that not all adolescents respond to ethnic victimization incidents in the same way. Instead, and in line with the literature on general victimization (Reijntjes et al.,
2016), four defender subgroups were identified: victim-oriented defenders (41.3%), non-defenders (25.4%), hybrid defenders (23.5%) and bully-oriented defenders (9.8%). While victim-oriented defenders show high levels of comforting the victim, but low levels of confronting the bully, non-defenders show low levels on both variables. Hybrid defenders show high levels on both variables and bully-oriented defenders show high levels of confronting the bully, but low levels of comforting the victim. Compared with other studies focusing on general victimization (Reijntjes et al.,
2016; Yun,
2020), the non-defender group was smaller in the present study, but in line with these previous studies victim-oriented defenders were a larger subgroup compared with hybrid (Reijntjes et al.,
2016) and bully-oriented defenders (Reijntjes et al.,
2016; Yun,
2020).
It is possible the smaller number of non-defenders that were found in the present study stem from different approaches of measurement. In the present study, adolescents were asked about their most likely reactions after presenting them a short incident covering foreign appearance, ethnic background and religion, as possible reasons for ethnic victimization. Conversely, in previous studies, youth were asked to evaluate their peers’ actual defending behaviors (Reijntjes et al.,
2016) or to report their own defending behaviors (Yun,
2020). Thus, it is possible that the measurement approach of the present study produced some kind of desirability bias. Another explanation might be related to societal influences on moral development. It is possible that early adolescents in Sweden may evaluate ethnicity-based victimization as more unfair, morally wrong, or devastating than non-ethnicity-based victimization compared to early adolescents residing in other countries because of a strong emphasis on equality and the high-level endorsement of anti-discrimination policies in Sweden compared to other countries (MIPEX,
2020). If this holds true, early adolescents residing in Sweden might be more motivated to defend targets of ethnic victimization compared with early adolescents residing in other countries which in turn might explain why the non-defender subgroup was smaller in the present study. Future research could illuminate this possibility.
In line with previous research (Reijntjes et al.,
2016; Yun,
2020), it was also found that more early adolescents are victim-oriented defenders compared to hybrid defenders or bully-oriented defenders. One possible explanation could be that when adolescents make a decision about supporting their victimized peers, they might perceive direct confrontation with the bully as riskier. Consequently, the majority of them might adopt strategies that involve defending the victim indirectly. This way, they convey an implicit message to the bully regarding the unacceptability of the behavior without directly challenging the bully. While a classical study of naturalistic observations of peer interventions on school playgrounds (Hawkins et al.,
2001) found that defenders adopting victim-oriented strategies were as effective as those adopting bully-oriented interventions in stopping general peer victimization, future research should explore which subgroup of defenders are more likely to be effective in stopping ethnic victimization incidents at schools.
Importantly, gender differences were also found, indicating that girls are more often hybrid defenders whereas boys are more likely to be bully-oriented defenders and non-defenders. No gender differences were found for victim-oriented defenders. It is possible that the higher levels of perspective taking among girls might explain their higher involvement in hybrid defending and lower involvement in non-defending. Immigrant status also mattered for defender group membership: Swedish adolescents were more likely to be non-defenders and less likely to be hybrid defenders. Adolescents with immigrant background, on the other hand, were more likely to be hybrid defenders and less likely to be non-defenders. Lastly, adolescents with mixed background were significantly less likely to be bully-oriented defenders. Adolescents with immigrant background and mixed background undergo similar migration experiences. It is possible that they develop an understanding and empathy for each other’s encounters in the host society. This augmented awareness may help them become more responsive to negative events and therefore motivate them to adopt different strategies to defend their peers who are facing ethnic victimization.
Characteristics of Defender Subgroups
The second main discovery of the present study is the identification of meaningful differences between the four defender subgroups regarding their socio-cognitive skills, perceptions about immigrants, and how they perceive their class context. Supporting the expectation and previous research on general bullying (e.g., Espelage et al.,
2012; Pozzoli et al.,
2017) and ethnic bullying (e.g., Abbot & Cameron,
2014; Gönültaş & Mulvey,
2023), it was found that hybrid and victim-oriented subgroups have higher levels of perspective taking skills compared to non-defenders. Interestingly, no difference was observed between bully-oriented defenders and non-defenders. As highlighted by the theory of prosocial moral behavior and development (Hoffman,
2001), perspective taking skills and empathy may enable young people to easily recognize the stress of victims. Assumed distress from victims may evoke a desire to alleviate the distress, thereby fostering adolescents’ involvement in defending behaviors. This could be one of the possible explanations of why hybrid and victim-oriented defenders have a higher level of perspective taking skills than bully-oriented defenders and non-defenders. Further, instead of focusing on the victim’s point of view, bully-oriented defenders may evaluate ethnic victimization through the lenses of fairness and social justice in their moral judgments. Thus, they may believe confronting the perpetrator directly is the most effective means to put an end to victimization and are unafraid of displaying their reactions.
Supporting the premises of the developmental intergroup approach (Bigler & Liben,
2006) and empirical studies (e.g., Bayram Özdemir et al.,
2022; Gönültaş & Mulvey,
2023), the findings showed that adolescents’ defending behaviors are related to their attitudes toward immigrants. Specifically, it was found that hybrid defenders have the highest positive attitudes toward immigrants and are followed by the victim-oriented defender group. As expected, non-defenders had the lowest level of positive attitudes toward immigrants but did not significantly differ from bully-oriented defenders. It is likely that hybrid and victim-oriented defenders might perceive ethnic and cultural diversity as an opportunity to gain new perspectives rather than as a threat. Relatedly, they may have sufficient internal motivation to place themselves in a potentially risky position. On the other hand, non-defenders may find comfort in the belief that the world is just (i.e., Dalbert,
2009). They may therefore think that victims deserve to be treated in this way. Such feelings and viewpoints may impede these adolescents’ moral judgments about problematic inter-group relationships, leading them to choose not to intervene with the intention of defending victims.
Another important conclusion to draw from the findings is that adolescents’ perceptions of the class climate seem to be related with how they respond to ethnic victimization incidents. Supporting the arguments of social-ecological theory of peer victimization (Swearer & Espelage,
2010) and expanding previous research (e.g., Bayram Özdemir et al.,
2022), it was found that all three defender subgroups were more likely to perceive their classroom climate as cooperative and socially cohesive compared to non-defenders. Further, hybrid defenders have a more positive perception about inter-ethnic norms in their class than victim-oriented defenders, but no difference was observed across hybrid and bully-oriented defenders. This finding underscores the significance of the classroom’s social climate in adopting more direct strategies (such as asking the bully to stop) to intervene in ethnic victimization incidents. It is possible that when youth are surrounded by peers who value diversity and embrace differences, they might find the courage to confront the bully because this supportive environment could decrease the potential costs associated with defending. Alternately, it is also plausible that inclusive and diversity-promoting class norms may foster the development of social-conventional reasoning in moral judgment among adolescents (i.e., ‘My group thinks it is not OK to bully someone because of her/his ethnic background’; Palmer et al.,
2015). Through such reasoning, adolescents may perceive the cost of defending as low and consequently may choose to take an action. This finding, in fact, align with the broader bullying literature, suggesting that youth are more inclined to take on the role of defender when they are in a class context that devalues bullying (Lucas-Molina et al.,
2018), emphasizes anti-bullying norms (Thornberg et al.,
2022), and associates bullying with social costs (Peets et al.,
2015). Alongside broader class norms, peer norms might play an important role in shaping adolescents’ values and defending behaviors during this developmental period. Despite some null findings (Gönültaş & Mulvey,
2021), research demonstrated that adolescents’ perceptions of their peers’ attitudes toward immigrants associated with a higher willingness to include victimized minority peers in their peer groups (Gönültaş & Mulvey,
2023) and a greater likelihood of standing against bias-based bullying (Hitti et al.,
2023). Future studies should examine norms within adolescents’ close social networks to gain a deeper understanding of how norms regarding diversity in social relationships might influence adolescents’ defending behaviors.
Contrary to the expectation and previous research (Caravita et al.,
2009), adolescents’ social status in peer contexts was not associated with their defending behaviors. All the four groups have similar levels of acceptance scores. Two alternative explanations can be provided for this finding. First, the role of overall peer acceptance across the four groups of defenders was investigated rather than differentiating peer acceptance by same versus cross ethnic peers. In fact, literature showed that bystanders’ judgments and responses in ethnic victimization differ based on whether the victim is an in- or outgroup member (Palmer et al.,
2022). Given that overall peer acceptance seems not to play an important role on adolescents’ defending strategies, future studies should examine this relation by differentiating peer acceptance into intra- and interethnic acceptance. Another possible explanation could be that while defenders put themselves in a risky position by defending their victimized peers, they might not solely rely on the size of their social network. The characteristics of adolescents’ peer context (e.g., peers’ attitudes toward immigrants; van Zalk et al.,
2013) and adolescents’ perceptions about the quality of their peer relations might contribute to their defending behaviors to a greater extent than their social status per se. It is possible that when early adolescents believe that they are surrounded by trusted peers, they may have the courage to challenge the bully because they may feel secure that they will be defended if they become the next target. Supporting this argument, literature showed that early adolescents (Jenkins & Nickerson,
2017) and adolescents (Evans & Smokowski,
2015) who perceived high level of support from their classmates were more likely to engage in prosocial bystander behavior in bullying situations. Therefore, these unmeasured factors might provide an explanation about the role of having secure and supportive social network in different defender groups. In sum, future research is needed to empirically examine these conceptual arguments, and to have a broader understanding about the role of peer relations on adolescents’ defending behaviors in ethnic victimization incidents.
Study Limitations and Future Research
Given some noteworthy limitations of the present study, there are plenty of avenues for future research. First, adolescents’ intended defending behaviors were examined by using a hypothetical scenario about ethnic victimization. Although a similar method has already been used in previous studies (Gönültaş & Mulvey,
2021), it is well known that self-reported intentions are only modestly associated with actual reactions (Ajzen,
2005). Hence, future studies could examine to what extent the present findings hold true in real life ethnic victimization events. Second, the stem question used to measure adolescents’ defending behaviors combined various forms of victimization, such as foreign appearance, ethnic background, and religion, to comprehensively explore their responses to victimization stemming from different facets of their peers’ immigrant backgrounds. Although this approach has some value as the perceived reasons for ethnic victimization among defenders may vary depending on the situation, this approach may lack the specificity needed to address each form of victimization separately. Therefore, future research could consider examining different forms of ethnic victimization separately. Third, adolescents were grouped based on their parents’ country origin (i.e., born in versus outside of Sweden): Swedish adolescents, adolescents with mixed background, and adolescents with immigrant background. However, this approach may lack specificity as it treats adolescents with immigrant and mixed backgrounds as relatively homogeneous groups. Examining heterogeneity among adolescents based on factors such as their country origin, the reasons for migration, and their length of stay in the host country would provide a more nuanced understanding of defending behaviors and subgroup differences among adolescents originating from diverse backgrounds. Although large samples are needed to be able to differentiate these many possible sub-groups, such a differentiation could be an important contribution to the literature, especially considering that minority adolescents (i.e., Pakistani) were found to be victimized not only by their native peers but also certain ethnic groups (i.e., Hindu) (Eslea & Mukhtar,
2000). Thus, it is reasonable that adolescents belonging to certain religious or ethnic background groups might defend each other more. Fourth, two single items were used to measure each intended defending behavior. Such a measurement approach might limit the ability to capture nuances as well as variability in defending behaviors. Measuring different aspects of defending with multiple items is therefore suggested in future studies for a more thorough examination of defending behaviors among adolescents. Fifth, adolescents defined as non-defenders may consist of further subgroups, such as those who show high levels of reinforcing behaviors or those who show high levels of passive bystander behaviors. Future research is needed to examine possible subgroups within the non-defender group. Sixth, the longitudinal stability of the identified defender groups remains unknown. Longitudinal studies following adolescents over several years could illuminate these patterns. Seventh, the questionnaire did not disclose the gender of the bully when measuring adolescents’ defending behaviors. Future studies could consider specifying the gender of the bully to investigate its potential influence on respondents’ perceptions and responses. Last but not least, future studies could consider teachers’ inter-ethnic norms, because they have been found to be associated with defending behaviors (Priest et al.,
2021).